How do animals experience pain Robyn J. Crook

Humans know the surprising prick
of a needle,

the searing pain of a stubbed toe,

and the throbbing of a toothache.

We can identify many types of pain
and have multiple ways of treating it.

But what about other species?

How do the animals all around us
experience pain?

It’s important that we find out.

We keep animals as pets,

they enrich our environment,

we farm many species for food,

and we use them in experiments
to advance science and human health.

Animals are clearly important to us,

so it’s equally important that we avoid
causing them unnecessary pain.

For animals that are similar to us,
like mammals,

it’s often obvious when they’re hurting.

But there’s a lot that isn’t obvious,

like whether pain relievers that work
on us also help them.

And the more different
an animal is from us,

the harder it is to understand
their experience.

How do you tell whether
a shrimp is in pain?

A snake?

A snail?

In vertebrates, including humans,

pain can be split
into two distinct processes.

In first, nerves and the skin sense
something harmful

and communicate that information
to the spinal cord.

There, motor neurons activate movements

that make us rapidly
jerk away from the threat.

This is the physical recognition of harm
called nociception,

and nearly all animals,

even those with very simple
nervous systems,

experience it.

Without this ability, animals would be
unable to avoid harm

and their survival would be threatened.

The second part is the conscious
recognition of harm.

In humans, this occurs when the sensory
neurons in our skin

make a second round of connections
via the spinal cord to the brain.

There, millions of neurons in multiple
regions create the sensations of pain.

For us, this is a very complex experience
associated with emotions like fear,

panic,

and stress,

which we can communicate to others.

But it’s harder to know exactly
how animals experience

this part of the process

because most them can’t show us
what they feel.

However, we get clues from observing
how animals behave.

Wild, hurt animals are known
to nurse their wounds,

make noises to show their distress,

and become reclusive.

In the lab, scientists have discovered
that animals like chickens and rats

will self-administer pain-reducing
drugs if they’re hurting.

Animals also avoid situations where
they’ve been hurt before,

which suggests awareness of threats.

We’ve reached the point that research
has made us so sure

that vertebrates recognize pain

that it’s illegal in many countries
to needlessly harm these animals.

But what about other types of animals
like invertebrates?

These animals aren’t legally protected,

partly because their behaviors
are harder to read.

We can make good guesses
about some of them,

like oysters,

worms,

and jellyfish.

These are examples of animals
that either lack a brain

or have a very simple one.

So an oyster may recoil when squirted
with lemon juice, for instance,

because of nociception.

But with such a simple nervous system,

it’s unlikely to experience
the conscious part of pain.

Other invertebrate animals
are more complicated, though,

like the octopus,

which has a sophisticated brain

and is thought to be one of the most
intelligent invertebrate animals.

Yet, in many countries, people continue
the practice of eating live octopus.

We also boil live crawfish, shrimp,
and crabs

even though we don’t really know
how they’re affected either.

This poses an ethical problem

because we may be causing these animals
unnecessary suffering.

Scientific experimentation,
though controversial, gives us some clues.

Tests on hermit crabs show that they’ll
leave an undesirable shell

if they’re zapped with electricity

but stay if it’s a good shell.

And octopi that may originally curl up
an injured arm to protect it

will risk using it to catch prey.

That suggests that these animals make
value judgements around sensory input

instead of just reacting
reflexively to harm.

Meanwhile, crabs have been known
to repeatedly rub a spot on their bodies

where they’ve received an electric shock.

And even sea slugs flinch

when they know they’re about
to receive a noxious stimulus.

That means they have some memory
of physical sensations.

We still have a lot to learn about
animal pain.

As our knowledge grows,

it may one day allow us to live in a world
where we don’t cause pain needlessly.