How do animals experience pain Robyn J. Crook

Humans know the surprising prick
of a needle,

the searing pain of a stubbed toe,

and the throbbing of a toothache.

We can identify many types of pain
and have multiple ways of treating it.

But what about other species?

How do the animals all around us
experience pain?

It’s important that we find out.

We keep animals as pets,

they enrich our environment,

we farm many species for food,

and we use them in experiments
to advance science and human health.

Animals are clearly important to us,

so it’s equally important that we avoid
causing them unnecessary pain.

For animals that are similar to us,
like mammals,

it’s often obvious when they’re hurting.

But there’s a lot that isn’t obvious,

like whether pain relievers that work
on us also help them.

And the more different
an animal is from us,

the harder it is to understand
their experience.

How do you tell whether
a shrimp is in pain?

A snake?

A snail?

In vertebrates, including humans,

pain can be split
into two distinct processes.

In first, nerves and the skin sense
something harmful

and communicate that information
to the spinal cord.

There, motor neurons activate movements

that make us rapidly
jerk away from the threat.

This is the physical recognition of harm
called nociception,

and nearly all animals,

even those with very simple
nervous systems,

experience it.

Without this ability, animals would be
unable to avoid harm

and their survival would be threatened.

The second part is the conscious
recognition of harm.

In humans, this occurs when the sensory
neurons in our skin

make a second round of connections
via the spinal cord to the brain.

There, millions of neurons in multiple
regions create the sensations of pain.

For us, this is a very complex experience
associated with emotions like fear,

panic,

and stress,

which we can communicate to others.

But it’s harder to know exactly
how animals experience

this part of the process

because most them can’t show us
what they feel.

However, we get clues from observing
how animals behave.

Wild, hurt animals are known
to nurse their wounds,

make noises to show their distress,

and become reclusive.

In the lab, scientists have discovered
that animals like chickens and rats

will self-administer pain-reducing
drugs if they’re hurting.

Animals also avoid situations where
they’ve been hurt before,

which suggests awareness of threats.

We’ve reached the point that research
has made us so sure

that vertebrates recognize pain

that it’s illegal in many countries
to needlessly harm these animals.

But what about other types of animals
like invertebrates?

These animals aren’t legally protected,

partly because their behaviors
are harder to read.

We can make good guesses
about some of them,

like oysters,

worms,

and jellyfish.

These are examples of animals
that either lack a brain

or have a very simple one.

So an oyster may recoil when squirted
with lemon juice, for instance,

because of nociception.

But with such a simple nervous system,

it’s unlikely to experience
the conscious part of pain.

Other invertebrate animals
are more complicated, though,

like the octopus,

which has a sophisticated brain

and is thought to be one of the most
intelligent invertebrate animals.

Yet, in many countries, people continue
the practice of eating live octopus.

We also boil live crawfish, shrimp,
and crabs

even though we don’t really know
how they’re affected either.

This poses an ethical problem

because we may be causing these animals
unnecessary suffering.

Scientific experimentation,
though controversial, gives us some clues.

Tests on hermit crabs show that they’ll
leave an undesirable shell

if they’re zapped with electricity

but stay if it’s a good shell.

And octopi that may originally curl up
an injured arm to protect it

will risk using it to catch prey.

That suggests that these animals make
value judgements around sensory input

instead of just reacting
reflexively to harm.

Meanwhile, crabs have been known
to repeatedly rub a spot on their bodies

where they’ve received an electric shock.

And even sea slugs flinch

when they know they’re about
to receive a noxious stimulus.

That means they have some memory
of physical sensations.

We still have a lot to learn about
animal pain.

As our knowledge grows,

it may one day allow us to live in a world
where we don’t cause pain needlessly.

人类知道令人惊讶的
针刺,

脚趾头的灼痛,

以及牙痛的悸动。

我们可以识别多种类型的疼痛
并有多种治疗方法。

但是其他物种呢?

我们周围的动物如何
体验疼痛?

我们找出来很重要。

我们饲养动物作为宠物,

它们丰富了我们的环境,

我们养殖了许多物种作为食物

,我们在实验中使用它们
来促进科学和人类健康。

动物显然对我们很重要,

所以我们避免
给它们带来不必要的痛苦同样重要。

对于与我们相似的动物,
例如哺乳动物,

它们受伤时通常很明显。

但是还有很多不明显的,

比如
对我们有效的止痛药是否也能帮助他们。

动物与

我们越不同,就越难理解
它们的经历。

如何
判断虾是否疼痛?

一条蛇?

蜗牛?

在包括人类在内的脊椎动物中,

疼痛可以
分为两个不同的过程。

首先,神经和皮肤会感觉到
有害的东西

并将这些信息传达
给脊髓。

在那里,运动神经元激活

使我们迅速
摆脱威胁的运动。

这是对伤害的物理识别,
称为伤害感受

,几乎所有动物,

即使是那些神经系统非常简单的动物
,都会

体验到它。

如果没有这种能力,动物将
无法避免伤害

,它们的生存将受到威胁。

第二部分是有意识地
承认伤害。

在人类中,当
我们皮肤中的感觉神经元

通过脊髓与大脑进行第二轮连接时,就会发生这种情况。

在那里,多个区域的数百万个神经元
产生疼痛感。

对我们来说,这是一种
与恐惧、恐慌和压力等情绪相关的非常复杂的体验

,我们可以与他人交流。

但很难确切地
知道动物是如何体验

这部分过程的,

因为大多数动物无法向我们
展示它们的感受。

然而,我们从
观察动物的行为中获得线索。

众所周知,野生、受伤的动物
会护理它们的伤口,

发出声音来表达它们的痛苦,

并变得隐居。

在实验室中,科学家们发现
,像鸡和老鼠这样的动物在受伤时

会自行服用减轻疼痛的
药物。

动物也会避免
以前受过伤害的情况,

这表明他们意识到了威胁。

我们已经到了这样的地步,
研究使我们

确信脊椎动物能够认识到疼痛

,以至于在许多
国家不必要地伤害这些动物是非法的。

但是其他类型的动物,
如无脊椎动物呢?

这些动物不受法律保护,

部分原因是它们的
行为难以理解。

我们可以
对其中一些做出很好的猜测,

比如牡蛎、

蠕虫

和水母。

这些是缺乏大脑

或非常简单的动物的例子。

因此,例如,由于伤害感受,牡蛎在喷上柠檬汁时可能会后退

但是有了这样一个简单的神经系统,

它不太可能体验
到疼痛的意识部分。 不过,

其他无脊椎动物
则更为复杂,

比如章鱼,

它拥有复杂的大脑

,被认为是最
聪明的无脊椎动物之一。

然而,在许多国家,人们继续
吃活章鱼的做法。

我们还煮活的小龙虾、虾
和螃蟹

,尽管我们也不
知道它们是如何受到影响的。

这带来了一个伦理问题,

因为我们可能会给这些动物造成
不必要的痛苦。

科学实验
虽然有争议,但也为我们提供了一些线索。

对寄居蟹的测试表明,如果它们被电击,它们会
留下一个不受欢迎的外壳

但如果它是一个好的外壳,它们会留下来。

章鱼原本可能会
蜷缩受伤的手臂来保护它,但它

会冒着用它来捕捉猎物的风险。

这表明这些动物会
围绕感官输入做出价值判断,

而不仅仅是
对伤害做出反射性反应。

与此同时,众所周知,螃蟹
会反复摩擦身体上触电的部位

甚至海蛞蝓

在知道自己
即将受到有害刺激时也会畏缩。

这意味着他们
对身体感觉有一些记忆。

关于动物的疼痛,我们还有很多需要了解

随着我们知识的增长,

有一天我们可能会生活在一个
不会造成不必要的痛苦的世界里。