How your brain decides what is beautiful Anjan Chatterjee

It’s 1878.

Sir Francis Galton
gives a remarkable talk.

He’s speaking to the anthropologic
institute of Great Britain and Ireland.

Known for his pioneering work
in human intelligence,

Galton is a brilliant polymath.

He’s an explorer,

an anthropologist,

a sociologist,

a psychologist

and a statistician.

He’s also a eugenist.

In this talk,

he presents a new technique
by which he can combine photographs

and produce composite portraits.

This technique could be used
to characterize different types of people.

Galton thinks that if he combines
photographs of violent criminals,

he will discover the face of criminality.

But to his surprise,

the composite portrait that he produces

is beautiful.

Galton’s surprising finding
raises deep questions:

What is beauty?

Why do certain configurations of line
and color and form excite us so?

For most of human history,

these questions have been approached
using logic and speculation.

But in the last few decades,

scientists have addressed
the question of beauty

using ideas from evolutionary psychology
and tools of neuroscience.

We’re beginning to glimpse
the why and the how of beauty,

at least in terms of what it means
for the human face and form.

And in the process,

we’re stumbling upon some surprises.

When it comes to seeing
beauty in each other,

while this decision is certainly
subjective for the individual,

it’s sculpted by factors that contribute
to the survival of the group.

Many experiments have shown

that a few basic parameters contribute
to what makes a face attractive.

These include averaging, symmetry
and the effects of hormones.

Let’s take each one of these in turn.

Galton’s finding

that composite or average faces
are typically more attractive

than each individual face
that contributes to the average

has been replicated many times.

This laboratory finding fits
with many people’s intuitions.

Average faces represent
the central tendencies of a group.

People with mixed features
represent different populations,

and presumably harbor
greater genetic diversity

and adaptability to the environment.

Many people find mixed-race
individuals attractive

and inbred families less so.

The second factor that contributes
to beauty is symmetry.

People generally find symmetric faces
more attractive than asymmetric ones.

Developmental abnormalities
are often associated with asymmetries.

And in plants, animals and humans,

asymmetries often arise
from parasitic infections.

Symmetry, it turns out,

is also an indicator of health.

In the 1930s,

a man named Maksymilian Faktorowicz

recognized the importance
of symmetry for beauty

when he designed the beauty micrometer.

With this device,

he could measure minor asymmetric flaws

which he could then make up for
with products he sold from his company,

named brilliantly
after himself, Max Factor,

which, as you know,
is one of the world’s most famous brands

for “make up.”

The third factor that contributes
to facial attractiveness

is the effect of hormones.

And here, I need to apologize
for confining my comments

to heterosexual norms.

But estrogen and testosterone
play important roles

in shaping features
that we find attractive.

Estrogen produces features
that signal fertility.

Men typically find women attractive

who have elements
of both youth and maturity.

A face that’s too baby-like might
mean that the girl is not yet fertile,

so men find women attractive

who have large eyes,
full lips and narrow chins

as indicators of youth,

and high cheekbones
as an indicator of maturity.

Testosterone produces features
that we regard as typically masculine.

These include heavier brows,

thinner cheeks

and bigger, squared-off jaws.

But here’s a fascinating irony.

In many species,

if anything,

testosterone suppresses the immune system.

So the idea that testosterone-infused
features are a fitness indicator

doesn’t really make a whole lot of sense.

Here, the logic is turned on its head.

Instead of a fitness indicator,

scientists invoke a handicap principle.

The most commonly cited
example of a handicap

is the peacock’s tail.

This beautiful but cumbersome tail
doesn’t exactly help the peacock

avoid predators

and approach peahens.

Why should such an extravagant
appendage evolve?

Even Charles Darwin,

in an 1860 letter to Asa Gray wrote

that the sight of the peacock’s tail
made him physically ill.

He couldn’t explain it
with his theory of natural selection,

and out of this frustration,

he developed the theory
of sexual selection.

On this account,

the display of the peacock’s tail
is about sexual enticement,

and this enticement means
it’s more likely the peacock will mate

and have offspring.

Now, the modern twist
on this display argument

is that the peacock is also
advertising its health to the peahen.

Only especially fit organisms
can afford to divert resources

to maintaining such
an extravagant appendage.

Only especially fit men can afford
the price that testosterone levies

on their immune system.

And by analogy, think of the fact

that only very rich men can afford
to pay more than $10,000 for a watch

as a display of their financial fitness.

Now, many people hear these kinds
of evolutionary claims

and think they mean that we somehow
are unconsciously seeking mates

who are healthy.

And I think this idea
is probably not right.

Teenagers and young adults are not exactly
known for making decisions

that are predicated on health concerns.

But they don’t have to be,

and let me explain why.

Imagine a population

in which people have three different
kinds of preferences:

for green, for orange and for red.

From their point of view,

these preferences have
nothing to do with health;

they just like what they like.

But if it were also the case
that these preferences are associated

with the different likelihood
of producing offspring –

let’s say in a ratio of 3:2:1 –

then in the first generation,

there would be 3 greens
to 2 oranges to 1 red,

and in each subsequent generation,

the proportion of greens increase,

so that in 10 generations,

98 percent of this population
has a green preference.

Now, a scientist coming in
and sampling this population

discovers that green
preferences are universal.

So the point about this little
abstract example

is that while preferences
for specific physical features

can be arbitrary for the individual,

if those features are heritable

and they are associated
with a reproductive advantage,

over time,

they become universal for the group.

So what happens in the brain
when we see beautiful people?

Attractive faces activate
parts of our visual cortex

in the back of the brain,

an area called the fusiform gyrus,

that is especially tuned
to processing faces,

and an adjacent area called
the lateral occipital complex,

that is especially attuned
to processing objects.

In addition,

attractive faces activate parts
of our reward and pleasure centers

in the front and deep in the brain,

and these include areas
that have complicated names,

like the ventral striatum,

the orbitofrontal cortex

and the ventromedial prefrontal cortex.

Our visual brain that is tuned
to processing faces

interacts with our pleasure centers

to underpin the experience of beauty.

Amazingly, while we all
engage with beauty,

without our knowledge,

beauty also engages us.

Our brains respond to attractive faces

even when we’re not thinking about beauty.

We conducted an experiment
in which people saw a series of faces,

and in one condition,

they had to decide if a pair of faces
were the same or a different person.

Even in this condition,

attractive faces drove neural activity
robustly in their visual cortex,

despite the fact that they were thinking
about a person’s identity

and not their beauty.

Another group similarly found
automatic responses to beauty

within our pleasure centers.

Taken together, these studies suggest

that our brain automatically
responds to beauty

by linking vision and pleasure.

These beauty detectors, it seems,

ping every time we see beauty,

regardless of whatever else
we might be thinking.

We also have a “beauty is good”
stereotype embedded in the brain.

Within the orbitofrontal cortex,

there’s overlapping neural activity

in response to beauty and to goodness,

and this happens even when people
aren’t explicitly thinking

about beauty or goodness.

Our brains seem to reflexively
associate beauty and good.

And this reflexive association
may be the biologic trigger

for the many social effects of beauty.

Attractive people receive
all kinds of advantages in life.

They’re regarded as more intelligent,

more trustworthy,

they’re given higher pay
and lesser punishments,

even when such judgments
are not warranted.

These kinds of observations
reveal beauty’s ugly side.

In my lab, we recently found

that people with minor facial
anomalies and disfigurements

are regarded as less good, less kind,

less intelligent, less competent
and less hardworking.

Unfortunately, we also have
a “disfigured is bad” stereotype.

This stereotype is probably
exploited and magnified

by images in popular media,

in which facial disfigurement
is often used as a shorthand

to depict someone of villainous character.

We need to understand
these kinds of implicit biases

if we are to overcome them

and aim for a society
in which we treat people fairly,

based on their behavior and not
on the happenstance of their looks.

Let me leave you with one final thought.

Beauty is a work in progress.

The so-called universal
attributes of beauty

were selected for during the almost
two million years of the Pleistocene.

Life was nasty, brutish
and a very long time ago.

The selection criteria
for reproductive success from that time

doesn’t really apply today.

For example,

death by parasite is not one
of the top ways that people die,

at least not in the technologically
developed world.

From antibiotics to surgery,

birth control to in vitro fertilization,

the filters for reproductive success
are being relaxed.

And under these relaxed conditions,

preference and trait combinations
are free to drift

and become more variable.

Even as we are profoundly
affecting our environment,

modern medicine
and technological innovation

is profoundly affecting

the very essence of what it means
to look beautiful.

The universal nature of beauty is changing

even as we’re changing the universe.

Thank you.

(Applause)

现在是 1878 年。

弗朗西斯·高尔顿爵士
发表了精彩的演讲。

他正在与
英国和爱尔兰的人类学研究所交谈。 高尔顿

以其在人类智能方面的开创性工作而闻名,他

是一位才华横溢的博学家。

他是探险家

、人类学家

、社会学家

、心理学家

和统计学家。

他也是一个优生学家。

在这次演讲中,

他展示了一种新技术,
通过该技术他可以组合照片

并制作合成肖像。

这种技术可
用于表征不同类型的人。

高尔顿认为,如果他结合
暴力罪犯的照片,

他会发现犯罪的面目。

但令他惊讶

的是,他制作的合成肖像

非常漂亮。

高尔顿的惊人发现
引发了深刻的问题:

什么是美?

为什么某些线条
、颜色和形式的配置让我们如此兴奋?

在人类历史的大部分时间里,

这些问题都是
用逻辑和推测来解决的。

但在过去的几十年里,

科学家们

利用进化心理学
和神经科学工具的思想解决了美的问题。

我们开始瞥见
美的原因和方式,

至少在
它对人的脸和形式意味着什么方面。

在这个过程中,

我们偶然发现了一些惊喜。

当谈到看到
彼此的美时,

虽然这个决定
对个人来说肯定是主观的,

但它是由有助于群体生存的因素塑造的

许多实验表明

,一些基本参数
有助于使面部具有吸引力。

这些包括平均、对称
和激素的影响。

让我们依次来看看这些。

高尔顿的发现

,即合成或平均面孔
通常


对平均水平有贡献的每张面孔更具吸引力,这一发现

已被多次复制。

这一实验室发现
符合许多人的直觉。

平均面孔代表
了一个群体的中心趋势。

具有混合特征的人
代表不同的人群

,可能
具有更大的遗传多样性

和对环境的适应性。

很多人觉得
混血儿有吸引力,

而近亲家庭则不那么有吸引力。

有助于美的第二个因素
是对称性。

人们通常发现对称的面孔
比不对称的面孔更有吸引力。

发育异常
通常与不对称有关。

在植物、动物和人类中,

不对称通常是
由寄生虫感染引起的。

事实证明,对称性

也是健康的一个指标。

1930 年代,

一位名叫 Maksymilian Faktorowicz 的人

在设计美感千分尺时认识到对称性对美感的重要性。

有了这个设备,

他可以测量微小的不对称缺陷

,然后他可以
用他公司出售的产品来弥补这些缺陷,该公司


他自己的名字命名,Max Factor

,正如你所知,
它是世界上最著名

的“制造”品牌之一 向上。”

第三个
影响面部

吸引力的因素是荷尔蒙的影响。

在这里,我需要
为将我的评论

局限于异性恋规范而道歉。

但雌激素和睾酮

在塑造
我们认为有吸引力的特征方面发挥着重要作用。

雌激素产生
表明生育能力的特征。

男性通常会发现

既有年轻又有成熟元素的女性很有吸引力。

一张太像婴儿的脸可能
意味着女孩还没有生育能力,

所以男人会觉得

眼睛大、
嘴唇丰满、下巴窄的女人很有吸引力,这

是年轻的标志,

而颧骨高
是成熟的标志。

睾酮
产生我们认为典型的男性特征。

这些包括较重的眉毛,

较薄的脸颊

和更大的方形下巴。

但这里有一个迷人的讽刺。

在许多物种中,

如果有的话,

睾酮会抑制免疫系统。

因此,睾酮注入
特征是健康指标的想法

并没有真正的意义。

在这里,逻辑颠倒了。 科学家没有

使用健康指标,而是使用

了障碍原理。

最常被引用
的障碍例子

是孔雀的尾巴。

这条美丽但笨重的尾巴
并不能完全帮助孔雀

避开掠食者

并接近孔雀。

如此奢侈的附属物为何要
进化?

甚至查尔斯·达尔文

在 1860 年给阿萨·格雷的一封信中

也写道,看到孔雀的尾巴
让他身体不适。

他无法
用他的自然选择理论来解释它

,出于这种挫败感,

他发展
了性选择理论。

因此,

孔雀尾巴的展示
是一种性诱惑,

而这种诱惑
意味着孔雀更有可能交配

并生育后代。

现在,
这个展示论点的现代转折

是孔雀也在
向孔雀宣传它的健康。

只有特别适合的生物
才能负担得起转移资源

来维持
这种奢侈的附属物。

只有特别健康的男性才能负担得起
睾丸激素

对其免疫系统征收的价格。

以此类推,

想想只有非常富有的人才能买
得起超过 10,000 美元的手表

,以显示他们的财务状况。

现在,许多人听到
这些进化论的说法,

并认为它们意味着我们不知何故
在不知不觉中寻找

健康的伴侣。

而且我认为这个
想法可能是不正确的。

青少年和年轻人并不完全
以做出

基于健康问题的决定而闻名。

但他们不一定是

,让我解释一下原因。

想象一个人群

,其中人们有三种
不同的偏好

:绿色、橙色和红色。

在他们看来,

这些偏好
与健康无关;

他们只是喜欢他们喜欢的东西。

但是,如果
这些偏好也与

产生后代的不同可能性相关——

假设比例为 3:2:1——

那么在第一代中

,将有 3 个绿色
对 2 个橙色对 1 红色,

并且在随后的每一代中,

绿色的比例都会增加,

因此在 10 代中,

98% 的人口
有绿色偏好。

现在,一位科学家进来
并对该人群进行抽样

发现,绿色
偏好是普遍存在的。

所以关于这个抽象的小例子的要点

是,虽然
对特定身体特征的偏好

对于个人来说可以是任意的,但

如果这些特征是可遗传的

并且它们
与繁殖优势相关联,

随着时间的推移,

它们会成为群体的普遍特征。

那么
当我们看到漂亮的人时,大脑会发生什么?

有吸引力的面孔会激活
我们大脑后部的部分视觉皮层

该区域称为梭状回

,特别
适合处理面部,

而相邻的区域
称为枕外侧复合体

,尤其
适合处理对象。

此外,

有吸引力的面孔会激活
我们

大脑前部和深处的部分奖励和愉悦中心,

其中包括
名称复杂的区域,

如腹侧纹状体

、眶额皮质

和腹内侧前额叶皮质。

我们
用于处理面孔的视觉大脑

与我们的快乐中心相互作用,

以支持美的体验。

令人惊讶的是,虽然我们都
与美有关,

但在我们不知情的情况下,

美也让我们参与其中。 即使

我们没有考虑美丽,我们的大脑也会对有吸引力的面孔做出反应

我们进行了一项
实验,人们看到一系列面孔

,在一种情况下,

他们必须确定一对面孔
是相同的人还是不同的人。

即使在这种情况下,

有吸引力的面孔也会
在他们的视觉皮层中强劲地驱动神经活动,

尽管他们考虑
的是一个人的身份

而不是他们的美貌。

另一组同样在我们的快乐中心发现了
对美的自动反应

总之,这些研究表明

,我们的大脑

通过将视觉和愉悦联系起来自动对美做出反应。

这些美容探测器似乎

每次我们看到美丽时

都会发出信号,不管
我们在想什么。

我们的大脑中也有“美就是好”的
刻板印象。

在眶额皮质内,

对美和善的反应存在重叠的神经活动,

即使
人们没有明确地

考虑美或善,也会发生这种情况。

我们的大脑似乎本能
地将美与善联系在一起。

这种反射性联想
可能是

美的许多社会影响的生物学触发因素。

有魅力的人会
在生活中获得各种优势。

他们被认为更聪明,

更值得信赖,

他们得到更高的薪水
和更少的惩罚,

即使这样的判断
是没有根据的。

这些观察
揭示了美丑陋的一面。

在我的实验室,我们最近

发现面部有轻微
异常和毁容的人

被认为不那么好、不那么善良、

不那么聪明、不那么有能力
和不那么勤奋。

不幸的是,我们也
有“毁容就是坏”的刻板印象。

这种刻板印象可能

流行媒体中的图像利用和放大,

其中面部
毁容经常被用作

描绘恶棍性格的人的速记。 如果我们要克服这些

隐性偏见,我们需要了解这些隐性偏见

并致力于建立一个
我们公平对待人们的社会,

基于他们的行为而不是
他们的外表的偶然性。

让我给你最后一个想法。

美丽是一项正在进行的工作。

所谓
的美的普遍属性是


近两百万年的更新世期间选择的。

生活是肮脏的,野蛮的,
而且是很久以前的事了。

从那时起
繁殖成功的选择标准在

今天并不适用。

例如,

寄生虫导致的死亡并不是
人们死亡的主要方式之一,

至少在技术
发达的世界中不是。

从抗生素到手术,从

节育到体外受精,

生殖成功的过滤
器正在放宽。

在这些宽松的条件下,

偏好和特征组合
可以自由漂移

并变得更加多变。

即使我们正在深刻地
影响我们的环境,

现代医学
和技术

创新也在深刻地影响

着美丽的本质。 即使我们

正在改变宇宙,美的普遍性

也在改变。

谢谢你。

(掌声)