Your brain hallucinates your conscious reality Anil Seth

Just over a year ago,

for the third time in my life,
I ceased to exist.

I was having a small operation,
and my brain was filling with anesthetic.

I remember a sense
of detachment and falling apart

and a coldness.

And then I was back,
drowsy and disoriented,

but definitely there.

Now, when you wake from a deep sleep,

you might feel confused about the time
or anxious about oversleeping,

but there’s always a basic sense
of time having passed,

of a continuity between then and now.

Coming round from
anesthesia is very different.

I could have been under
for five minutes, five hours,

five years or even 50 years.

I simply wasn’t there.

It was total oblivion.

Anesthesia –
it’s a modern kind of magic.

It turns people into objects,

and then, we hope, back again into people.

And in this process

is one of the greatest remaining
mysteries in science and philosophy.

How does consciousness happen?

Somehow, within each of our brains,

the combined activity
of many billions of neurons,

each one a tiny biological machine,

is generating a conscious experience.

And not just any conscious experience –

your conscious experience
right here and right now.

How does this happen?

Answering this question is so important

because consciousness
for each of us is all there is.

Without it there’s no world,

there’s no self,

there’s nothing at all.

And when we suffer, we suffer consciously

whether it’s through
mental illness or pain.

And if we can experience
joy and suffering,

what about other animals?

Might they be conscious, too?

Do they also have a sense of self?

And as computers get faster and smarter,

maybe there will come a point,
maybe not too far away,

when my iPhone develops
a sense of its own existence.

I actually think the prospects
for a conscious AI are pretty remote.

And I think this because
my research is telling me

that consciousness has less to do
with pure intelligence

and more to do with our nature
as living and breathing organisms.

Consciousness and intelligence
are very different things.

You don’t have to be smart to suffer,
but you probably do have to be alive.

In the story I’m going to tell you,

our conscious experiences
of the world around us,

and of ourselves within it,

are kinds of controlled hallucinations

that happen with, through
and because of our living bodies.

Now, you might have heard
that we know nothing

about how the brain and body
give rise to consciousness.

Some people even say it’s beyond
the reach of science altogether.

But in fact,

the last 25 years have seen an explosion
of scientific work in this area.

If you come to my lab
at the University of Sussex,

you’ll find scientists
from all different disciplines

and sometimes even philosophers.

All of us together trying to understand
how consciousness happens

and what happens when it goes wrong.

And the strategy is very simple.

I’d like you to think about consciousness

in the way that we’ve
come to think about life.

At one time, people thought
the property of being alive

could not be explained
by physics and chemistry –

that life had to be
more than just mechanism.

But people no longer think that.

As biologists got on with the job

of explaining the properties
of living systems

in terms of physics and chemistry –

things like metabolism,
reproduction, homeostasis –

the basic mystery of what life is
started to fade away,

and people didn’t propose
any more magical solutions,

like a force of life or an élan vital.

So as with life, so with consciousness.

Once we start explaining its properties

in terms of things happening
inside brains and bodies,

the apparently insoluble mystery
of what consciousness is

should start to fade away.

At least that’s the plan.

So let’s get started.

What are the properties of consciousness?

What should a science
of consciousness try to explain?

Well, for today I’d just like to think
of consciousness in two different ways.

There are experiences
of the world around us,

full of sights, sounds and smells,

there’s multisensory, panoramic,
3D, fully immersive inner movie.

And then there’s conscious self.

The specific experience
of being you or being me.

The lead character in this inner movie,

and probably the aspect of consciousness
we all cling to most tightly.

Let’s start with experiences
of the world around us,

and with the important idea
of the brain as a prediction engine.

Imagine being a brain.

You’re locked inside a bony skull,

trying to figure
what’s out there in the world.

There’s no lights inside the skull.
There’s no sound either.

All you’ve got to go on
is streams of electrical impulses

which are only indirectly related
to things in the world,

whatever they may be.

So perception –
figuring out what’s there –

has to be a process of informed guesswork

in which the brain combines
these sensory signals

with its prior expectations or beliefs
about the way the world is

to form its best guess
of what caused those signals.

The brain doesn’t hear sound or see light.

What we perceive is its best guess
of what’s out there in the world.

Let me give you a couple
of examples of all this.

You might have seen this illusion before,

but I’d like you to think
about it in a new way.

If you look at those two patches, A and B,

they should look to you to be
very different shades of gray, right?

But they are in fact
exactly the same shade.

And I can illustrate this.

If I put up a second version
of the image here

and join the two patches
with a gray-colored bar,

you can see there’s no difference.

It’s exactly the same shade of gray.

And if you still don’t believe me,

I’ll bring the bar across
and join them up.

It’s a single colored block of gray,
there’s no difference at all.

This isn’t any kind of magic trick.

It’s the same shade of gray,

but take it away again,
and it looks different.

So what’s happening here

is that the brain
is using its prior expectations

built deeply into the circuits
of the visual cortex

that a cast shadow dims
the appearance of a surface,

so that we see B as lighter
than it really is.

Here’s one more example,

which shows just how quickly
the brain can use new predictions

to change what we consciously experience.

Have a listen to this.

(Distorted voice)

Sounded strange, right?

Have a listen again
and see if you can get anything.

(Distorted voice)

Still strange.

Now listen to this.

(Recording) Anil Seth: I think Brexit
is a really terrible idea.

(Laughter)

Which I do.

So you heard some words there, right?

Now listen to the first sound again.
I’m just going to replay it.

(Distorted voice)

Yeah? So you can now hear words there.

Once more for luck.

(Distorted voice)

OK, so what’s going on here?

The remarkable thing is the sensory
information coming into the brain

hasn’t changed at all.

All that’s changed
is your brain’s best guess

of the causes of that sensory information.

And that changes
what you consciously hear.

All this puts the brain
basis of perception

in a bit of a different light.

Instead of perception depending largely
on signals coming into the brain

from the outside world,

it depends as much, if not more,

on perceptual predictions
flowing in the opposite direction.

We don’t just passively
perceive the world,

we actively generate it.

The world we experience
comes as much, if not more,

from the inside out

as from the outside in.

Let me give you
one more example of perception

as this active, constructive process.

Here we’ve combined immersive
virtual reality with image processing

to simulate the effects
of overly strong perceptual predictions

on experience.

In this panoramic video,
we’ve transformed the world –

which is in this case Sussex campus –

into a psychedelic playground.

We’ve processed the footage using
an algorithm based on Google’s Deep Dream

to simulate the effects
of overly strong perceptual predictions.

In this case, to see dogs.

And you can see
this is a very strange thing.

When perceptual
predictions are too strong,

as they are here,

the result looks very much
like the kinds of hallucinations

people might report in altered states,

or perhaps even in psychosis.

Now, think about this for a minute.

If hallucination is a kind
of uncontrolled perception,

then perception right here and right now
is also a kind of hallucination,

but a controlled hallucination

in which the brain’s predictions
are being reined in

by sensory information from the world.

In fact, we’re all
hallucinating all the time,

including right now.

It’s just that when we agree
about our hallucinations,

we call that reality.

(Laughter)

Now I’m going to tell you
that your experience of being a self,

the specific experience of being you,

is also a controlled hallucination
generated by the brain.

This seems a very strange idea, right?

Yes, visual illusions
might deceive my eyes,

but how could I be deceived
about what it means to be me?

For most of us,

the experience of being a person

is so familiar, so unified
and so continuous

that it’s difficult
not to take it for granted.

But we shouldn’t take it for granted.

There are in fact many different ways
we experience being a self.

There’s the experience of having a body

and of being a body.

There are experiences
of perceiving the world

from a first person point of view.

There are experiences
of intending to do things

and of being the cause of things
that happen in the world.

And there are experiences

of being a continuous
and distinctive person over time,

built from a rich set
of memories and social interactions.

Many experiments show,

and psychiatrists
and neurologists know very well,

that these different ways
in which we experience being a self

can all come apart.

What this means is
the basic background experience

of being a unified self is a rather
fragile construction of the brain.

Another experience,
which just like all others,

requires explanation.

So let’s return to the bodily self.

How does the brain generate
the experience of being a body

and of having a body?

Well, just the same principles apply.

The brain makes its best guess

about what is and what is not
part of its body.

And there’s a beautiful experiment
in neuroscience to illustrate this.

And unlike most neuroscience experiments,

this is one you can do at home.

All you need is one of these.

(Laughter)

And a couple of paintbrushes.

In the rubber hand illusion,

a person’s real hand is hidden from view,

and that fake rubber hand
is placed in front of them.

Then both hands are simultaneously
stroked with a paintbrush

while the person stares at the fake hand.

Now, for most people, after a while,

this leads to the very uncanny sensation

that the fake hand
is in fact part of their body.

And the idea is that the congruence
between seeing touch and feeling touch

on an object that looks like hand
and is roughly where a hand should be,

is enough evidence for the brain
to make its best guess

that the fake hand
is in fact part of the body.

(Laughter)

So you can measure
all kinds of clever things.

You can measure skin conductance
and startle responses,

but there’s no need.

It’s clear the guy in blue
has assimilated the fake hand.

This means that even experiences
of what our body is

is a kind of best guessing –

a kind of controlled
hallucination by the brain.

There’s one more thing.

We don’t just experience our bodies
as objects in the world from the outside,

we also experience them from within.

We all experience the sense
of being a body from the inside.

And sensory signals
coming from the inside of the body

are continually telling the brain
about the state of the internal organs,

how the heart is doing,
what the blood pressure is like,

lots of things.

This kind of perception,
which we call interoception,

is rather overlooked.

But it’s critically important

because perception and regulation
of the internal state of the body –

well, that’s what keeps us alive.

Here’s another version
of the rubber hand illusion.

This is from our lab at Sussex.

And here, people see
a virtual reality version of their hand,

which flashes red and back

either in time or out of time
with their heartbeat.

And when it’s flashing
in time with their heartbeat,

people have a stronger sense
that it’s in fact part of their body.

So experiences of having a body
are deeply grounded

in perceiving our bodies from within.

There’s one last thing
I want to draw your attention to,

which is that experiences of the body
from the inside are very different

from experiences of the world around us.

When I look around me,
the world seems full of objects –

tables, chairs, rubber hands,

people, you lot –

even my own body in the world,

I can perceive it
as an object from the outside.

But my experiences
of the body from within,

they’re not like that at all.

I don’t perceive my kidneys here,

my liver here,

my spleen …

I don’t know where my spleen is,

but it’s somewhere.

I don’t perceive my insides as objects.

In fact, I don’t experience them
much at all unless they go wrong.

And this is important, I think.

Perception of the internal
state of the body

isn’t about figuring out what’s there,

it’s about control and regulation –

keeping the physiological variables
within the tight bounds

that are compatible with survival.

When the brain uses predictions
to figure out what’s there,

we perceive objects
as the causes of sensations.

When the brain uses predictions
to control and regulate things,

we experience how well
or how badly that control is going.

So our most basic experiences
of being a self,

of being an embodied organism,

are deeply grounded in the biological
mechanisms that keep us alive.

And when we follow this idea
all the way through,

we can start to see
that all of our conscious experiences,

since they all depend on the same
mechanisms of predictive perception,

all stem from this basic
drive to stay alive.

We experience the world and ourselves

with, through and because of
our living bodies.

Let me bring things together step-by-step.

What we consciously see depends

on the brain’s best guess
of what’s out there.

Our experienced world
comes from the inside out,

not just the outside in.

The rubber hand illusion shows
that this applies to our experiences

of what is and what is not our body.

And these self-related predictions
depend critically on sensory signals

coming from deep inside the body.

And finally,

experiences of being an embodied self
are more about control and regulation

than figuring out what’s there.

So our experiences of the world
around us and ourselves within it –

well, they’re kinds
of controlled hallucinations

that have been shaped
over millions of years of evolution

to keep us alive in worlds
full of danger and opportunity.

We predict ourselves into existence.

Now, I leave you with three
implications of all this.

First, just as we can
misperceive the world,

we can misperceive ourselves

when the mechanisms
of prediction go wrong.

Understanding this opens many new
opportunities in psychiatry and neurology,

because we can finally
get at the mechanisms

rather than just treating the symptoms

in conditions like
depression and schizophrenia.

Second:

what it means to be me
cannot be reduced to or uploaded to

a software program running on a robot,

however smart or sophisticated.

We are biological, flesh-and-blood animals

whose conscious experiences
are shaped at all levels

by the biological mechanisms
that keep us alive.

Just making computers smarter
is not going to make them sentient.

Finally,

our own individual inner universe,

our way of being conscious,

is just one possible
way of being conscious.

And even human consciousness generally –

it’s just a tiny region in a vast space
of possible consciousnesses.

Our individual self and worlds
are unique to each of us,

but they’re all grounded
in biological mechanisms

shared with many other living creatures.

Now, these are fundamental changes

in how we understand ourselves,

but I think they should be celebrated,

because as so often in science,
from Copernicus –

we’re not at the center of the universe –

to Darwin –

we’re related to all other creatures –

to the present day.

With a greater sense of understanding

comes a greater sense of wonder,

and a greater realization

that we are part of
and not apart from the rest of nature.

And …

when the end of consciousness comes,

there’s nothing to be afraid of.

Nothing at all.

Thank you.

(Applause)

就在一年多以前

,我生命中第三次
不复存在。

我正在做一个小手术
,我的大脑充满了麻醉剂。

我记得有一种
分离感、分崩离析

和冰冷的感觉。

然后我回来了,
昏昏欲睡和迷失方向,

但肯定在那里。

现在,当你从沉睡中醒来时,

你可能会对时间感到困惑
或担心睡过头,

但总有一种基本
的时间已经过去

,从那时到现在的连续性。


麻醉中恢复过来是非常不同的。

我本可以在下面
呆五分钟、五个小时、

五年甚至五十年。

我根本不在那里。

完全被遗忘了。

麻醉——
这是一种现代的魔法。

它将人变成物体,

然后,我们希望,再次变成人。

在这个过程中,


科学和哲学中最大的谜团之一。

意识是如何发生的?

不知何故,在我们每个人的大脑中,

数十亿个神经元的联合活动,每个神经元

都是一个微型生物机器,

正在产生一种有意识的体验。

而不仅仅是任何有意识的体验——

你此时此地的有意识
体验。

这是怎么发生的?

回答这个问题非常重要,

因为
我们每个人的意识就是一切。

没有它,就没有世界,

没有自我,

什么都没有。

当我们受苦时,我们有意识地受苦,

无论是
精神疾病还是痛苦。

如果我们可以体验
快乐和痛苦,

那么其他动物呢?

他们可能也有意识吗?

他们也有自我意识吗?

随着计算机变得更快、更智能,

也许会出现一个点,
也许不会太远,

当我的 iPhone 发展
出对自己存在的感觉时。

实际上,我认为
有意识的人工智能的前景非常遥远。

我认为这是因为
我的研究告诉我

,意识
与纯粹的智力关系不大,

而更多地与我们
作为活生生的有机体的本性有关。

意识和智力
是非常不同的东西。

你不必很聪明才能受苦,
但你可能必须活着。

在我要告诉你的故事中,

我们对周围世界的有意识体验,

以及我们在其中的自我体验,

是一种受控的幻觉

,发生在、通过
和因为我们的活体。

现在,您可能已经
听说我们

对大脑和身体如何
产生意识一无所知。

有些人甚至说这
完全超出了科学的范围。

但事实上,

在过去的 25 年里
,这一领域的科学工作呈爆炸式增长。

如果你来到我
在苏塞克斯大学的实验室,

你会发现
来自所有不同学科的科学家

,有时甚至是哲学家。

我们所有人都试图
了解意识

是如何发生的,以及当它出错时会发生什么。

而且策略非常简单。

我希望你以

我们
思考生命的方式来思考意识。

曾几何时,人们认为
生命的特性无法

用物理和化学来解释

——生命必须
不仅仅是机械。

但人们不再这么认为了。

随着生物学家开始

从物理和化学角度解释生命系统的特性

——比如新陈代谢、
繁殖、体内平衡——

生命开始消失的基本谜团

,人们没有提出
任何 更神奇的解决方案,

如生命力或活力活力。

生命如此,意识亦如此。

一旦我们开始

根据大脑和身体内部发生的事情来解释它的特性,

关于什么是意识的表面上

无法解开的谜团应该开始消失。

至少这是计划。

所以让我们开始吧。

意识的属性是什么?

意识科学应该试图解释什么?

好吧,今天我只想
以两种不同的方式来思考意识。


我们周围世界的体验,

充满了视觉、声音和气味,

有多感官、全景、
3D、完全身临其境的内心电影。

然后是有意识的自我。

成为你或成为我的具体体验。

这部内心电影中的主角

,可能是
我们都紧紧抓住的意识方面。

让我们从
我们周围世界的经验开始,

以及
大脑作为预测引擎的重要思想。

想象自己是一个大脑。

你被锁在一个骨质的头骨里,

试图
弄清楚世界上有什么。

头骨内没有光。
也没有声音。

你所要做的
只是电脉冲流

,它们只是与
世界上的事物间接相关,

无论它们是什么。

因此,感知——
弄清楚那里有什么——

必须是一个明智的猜测过程,

在这个过程中,大脑将
这些感觉信号

与其对世界方式的先前期望或信念结合

起来,形成
对导致这些信号的最佳猜测。

大脑听不到声音或看不到光。

我们所感知的是它对
世界上存在的事物的最佳猜测。

让我
举几个例子。

你可能以前见过这种错觉,

但我希望你
以一种新的方式思考它。

如果你看看那两个补丁,A 和 B,

它们在你看来应该是
非常不同的灰色阴影,对吧?

但它们实际上
是完全相同的阴影。

我可以说明这一点。

如果我在这里放第二个版本
的图像


用灰色条连接两个补丁,

你可以看到没有区别。

它是完全相同的灰色阴影。

如果你仍然不相信我,

我会带酒吧过去
并加入他们的行列。

它是一个单一的灰色块,
完全没有区别。

这不是什么魔术。

它是相同的灰色阴影,

但再把它拿走
,它看起来就不同了。

所以这里发生的事情

是,大脑
正在利用其在视觉皮层

回路中深层构建的先前预期

,即投射阴影
使表面的外观变暗,

因此我们看到 B
比实际更轻。

这是另一个例子,

它显示了大脑可以多快地
使用新的预测

来改变我们有意识的体验。

听听这个。

(扭曲的声音)

听起来很奇怪,对吧?

再听一遍
,看看你能不能得到什么。

(扭曲的声音)

还是很奇怪。

现在听这个。

(录音) Anil Seth:我认为英国脱欧
是一个非常糟糕的主意。

(笑声)

我就是这么做的。

所以你在那里听到了一些话,对吧?

现在再听一下第一个声音。
我只是要重播它。

(扭曲的声音)

是吗? 所以你现在可以在那里听到文字。

再次祝你好运。

(扭曲的声音)

好的,那这里发生了什么事?

值得注意的是,
进入大脑的感觉信息

根本没有改变。

改变的
只是你的大脑

对感官信息原因的最佳猜测。

这会改变
你有意识地听到的内容。

所有这些都让大脑
感知的基础

有了一点不同。

感知不是主要
依赖于从外部世界进入大脑的信号

而是依赖于相反方向的感知预测,如果不是更多的话

我们不只是被动地
感知世界,

而是主动地创造它。

我们所体验的世界
,即使不是更多,

也是由内而外的。

让我再举
一个例子,将感知

作为这个积极的、建设性的过程。

在这里,我们将沉浸式
虚拟现实与图像处理相结合,


模拟过强的感知预测

对体验的影响。

在这段全景视频中,
我们将世界

——在本例中是苏塞克斯校园——

变成了一个迷幻的游乐场。

我们使用
基于 Google 的 Deep Dream 的算法处理了这些片段,


模拟过强的感知预测的影响。

在这种情况下,看狗。

你可以看到
这是一件非常奇怪的事情。

当感知
预测太强时,

就像他们在这里一样

,结果看起来很像

人们在改变状态

甚至精神病中可能报告的那种幻觉。

现在,考虑一下。

如果幻觉是
一种不受控制的知觉,

那么此时此地的知觉
也是一种幻觉,

而是一种受控制的幻觉

,其中大脑的预测

来自世界的感官信息所控制。

事实上,我们一直都在产生
幻觉,

包括现在。

只是当我们
同意我们的幻觉时,

我们称之为现实。

(笑声)

现在我要告诉你
,你成为自我

的体验,成为你的具体体验,

也是
大脑产生的受控幻觉。

这似乎是一个非常奇怪的想法,对吧?

是的,视觉错觉
可能会欺骗我的眼睛,

但我怎么会被我的意义所欺骗
呢?

对于我们大多数人来说,

作为一个人的经历

是如此熟悉、如此统一
和如此连续

,以至于很难
不认为这是理所当然的。

但我们不应该认为这是理所当然的。

事实上,
我们体验自我的方式有很多种。

有拥有身体和成为身体的体验

从第一人称视角感知世界的经历。


打算做某事的经验,

以及成为
世界上发生的事情的原因的经验。

随着时间的推移,有一些经历

会成为一个持续的
、与众不同的人,这些经历是

建立在丰富
的记忆和社交互动的基础上的。

许多实验表明

,精神病学家
和神经学家非常清楚,

我们体验成为自我的这些不同

方式都可以分开。

这意味着

成为统一自我的基本背景经验是大脑的相当
脆弱的构造。

另一种体验
,就像所有其他体验一样,

需要解释。

所以让我们回到身体的自我。

大脑如何产生
成为身体

和拥有身体的体验?

好吧,同样的原则也适用。

大脑会做出最好的

猜测,什么是
身体的一部分,什么不是身体的一部分。

神经科学中有一个漂亮的实验
来说明这一点。

与大多数神经科学实验不同,

这是您可以在家中进行的实验。

您所需要的只是其中之一。

(笑声)

还有几把画笔。

在橡胶手幻觉中,

一个人的真手被隐藏起来,

而那只假橡胶手
被放在他们面前。

然后双手同时
用画笔抚摸,

而人则盯着假手。

现在,对于大多数人来说,一段时间后,

这会导致一种非常不可思议的感觉

,即
假手实际上是他们身体的一部分。

这个想法是,

在一个看起来像手并且大致是手应该在的位置的物体上,看到触摸和感觉触摸之间的一致性

,足以证明大脑
做出最好的猜测

,即
假手实际上是 身体。

(笑声)

所以你可以测量
各种聪明的东西。

您可以测量皮肤电导
和惊吓反应,

但没有必要。

很明显,蓝衣人
已经同化了假手。

这意味着即使
是对我们身体的体验

也是一种最好的猜测——

一种大脑控制的
幻觉。

还有一件事。

我们不仅从外部体验我们的身体
作为世界中的物体,

我们还从内部体验它们。

我们都
从内部体验到成为一个身体的感觉。

来自身体内部的感觉信号

不断地告诉大脑
内部器官的状态

、心脏的状况、

血压的情况等等。

这种我们称之为内感受的知觉

是相当被忽视的。

但它至关重要,

因为
对身体内部状态的感知和调节——

嗯,这就是让我们活着的原因。


是橡胶手错觉的另一个版本。

这是来自我们在苏塞克斯的实验室。

在这里,人们看到
了他们手的虚拟现实版本,随着他们的心跳及时或不合时宜

地闪烁红色并返回

当它
随着他们的心跳而闪烁时,

人们会更强烈地
感觉到它实际上是他们身体的一部分。

因此,拥有身体的体验
深深植根

于从内部感知我们的身体。

我想提请您注意

的最后一件事是,
从内部对身体的

体验与对我们周围世界的体验非常不同。

当我环顾四周时
,世界似乎充满了物体——

桌子、椅子、橡胶手、

人、你们——

甚至我自己的身体在这个世界上,

我都可以
从外部将它视为一个物体。

但我
从内部对身体的体验,

根本不是那样的。

我不知道我的肾脏在

这里,我的肝脏,

我的脾脏……

我不知道我的脾脏在哪里,

但它在某个地方。

我不认为我的内心是物体。

事实上,
除非它们出错,否则我根本不会体验它们。

我认为这很重要。

对身体内部状态的感知

不是要弄清楚那里有什么,

而是要控制和调节——

将生理变量保持在

与生存相适应的严格范围内。

当大脑使用预测
来弄清楚那里有什么时,

我们将物体
视为感觉的原因。

当大脑使用预测
来控制和调节事物时,

我们会体验到
这种控制的好坏程度。

因此,我们
作为一个自我

、作为一个具身有机体的最基本体验

,深深植根于
维持我们生命的生物机制。

当我们一直遵循这个
想法时,

我们可以开始看到
我们所有的有意识体验,

因为它们都依赖于相同
的预测感知机制,

都源于这种
维持生命的基本动力。

我们

通过、通过和因为
我们的活体来体验世界和我们自己。

让我一步一步地把事情放在一起。

我们有意识地看到什么

取决于大脑
对外面有什么的最佳猜测。

我们所体验的世界
是由内而外的,

而不仅仅是由外而内

。橡胶手错觉表明
,这适用于我们

对什么是我们的身体和什么不是我们的身体的体验。

这些自我相关的预测
严重依赖于

来自身体深处的感觉信号。

最后,

成为一个具身自我的体验
更多的是关于控制和调节,而

不是弄清楚那里有什么。

所以我们对
周围世界和我们自己的体验——

嗯,它们是
一种受控的幻觉


经过数百万年的进化形成

,让我们在
充满危险和机遇的世界中生存。

我们预测自己的存在。

现在,我给你留下
这一切的三个含义。

首先,就像我们会
误解世界一样,当预测机制出错时,

我们也会误解自己

了解这一点
为精神病学和神经病学开辟了许多新的机会,

因为我们最终
可以了解这些机制,

而不仅仅是治疗

抑郁症和精神分裂症等疾病的症状。

第二:

作为我的意义
不能被简化为或上传到

运行在机器人上的软件程序,

无论它多么聪明或复杂。

我们是有血有肉的生物动物

,它们的意识体验
在各个层面都

受到维持我们生命的生物机制的影响

仅仅让计算机更
智能不会让它们有感知力。

最后,

我们自己的内在宇宙,

我们的意识方式,

只是一种可能
的意识方式。

甚至一般的人类意识——

它只是一个巨大的可能意识空间中的一个很小的区域

我们每个人的自我和世界
都是独一无二的,

但它们都基于

与许多其他生物共享的生物机制。

现在,这些是

我们如何理解自己的根本变化,

但我认为它们应该受到庆祝,

因为在科学中,
从哥白尼——

我们不是宇宙的中心——

到达尔文——

我们经常 与所有其他生物有关

——直到今天。

有了更伟大的理解感,

就会有更大的惊奇感,

以及更大的

意识到我们是
大自然的一部分,而不是与大自然的其他部分分开。

而且……

当意识终结时,

没有什么好害怕的。

什么都没有。

谢谢你。

(掌声)