What color is Tuesday Exploring synesthesia Richard E. Cytowic

Translator: Andrea McDonough
Reviewer: Jessica Ruby

Imagine a world

in which you see numbers and letters as colored

even though they’re printed in black,

in which music or voices trigger a swirl

of moving, colored shapes,

in which words and names fill your mouth

with unusual flavors.

Jail tastes like cold, hard bacon

while Derek tastes like earwax.

Welcome to synesthesia,

the neurological phenomenon

that couples two or more senses in 4% of the population.

A synesthete might not only hear my voice,

but also see it,

taste it,

or feel it as a physical touch.

Sharing the same root with anesthesia,

meaning no sensation,

synesthesia means joined sensation.

Having one type, such as colored hearing,

gives you a 50% chance of having a second,

third,

or fourth type.

One in 90 among us experience graphemes,

the written elements of language,

like letters,

numerals,

and punctuation marks,

as saturated with color.

Some even have gender or personality.

For Gail, 3 is athletic and sporty,

9 is a vain, elitist girl.

By contrast, the sound units of language,

or phonemes,

trigger synestetic tastes.

For James, college tastes like sausage,

as does message and similar words

with the -age ending.

Synesthesia is a trait, like having blue eyes,

rather than a disorder

because there’s nothing wrong.

In fact, all the extra hooks

endow synesthetes with superior memories.

For example, a girl runs into someone she met long ago.

“Let’s see, she had a green name.

D’s are green:

Debra,

Darby,

Dorothy,

Denise.

Yes! Her name is Denise!”

Once established in childhood,

pairings remain fixed for life.

Synesthetes inherit a biological propensity

for hyperconnecting brain neurons,

but then must be exposed to cultural artifacts,

such as calendars,

food names,

and alphabets.

The amazing thing is that a single nucleotide change

in the sequence of one’s DNA alters perception.

In this way, synesthesia provides a path

to understanding subjective differences,

how two people can see the same thing differently.

Take Sean, who prefers blue tasting food,

such as milk, oranges, and spinach.

The gene heightens normally occurring connections

between the taste area in his frontal lobe

and the color area further back.

But suppose in someone else

that the gene acted in non-sensory areas.

You would then have the ability to link

seemingly unrelated things,

which is the definition of metaphor,

seeing the similar in the dissimilar.

Not surprisingly, synesthesia is more common

in artists who excel at making metaphors,

like novelist Vladimir Nabokov,

painter David Hockney,

and composers Billy Joel

and Lady Gaga.

But why do the rest of us non-synesthetes

understand metaphors like “sharp cheese”

or “sweet person”?

It so happens that sight,

sound,

and movement

already map to one another so closely,

that even bad ventriloquists convince us

that the dummy is talking.

Movies, likewise, can convince us

that the sound is coming from the actors' mouths

rather than surrounding speakers.

So, inwardly, we’re all synesthetes,

outwardly unaware of the perceptual couplings

happening all the time.

Cross-talk in the brain is the rule,

not the exception.

And that sounds like a sweet deal to me!

译者:Andrea McDonough
审稿人:Jessica Ruby

想象一个

世界,你看到数字和字母是彩色的,

即使它们是黑色的

,其中音乐或声音会触发

移动的彩色形状的漩涡

,其中单词和名字会填满你的 嘴里

有不寻常的味道。

Jail 尝起来像冰冷的硬培根,

而 Derek 尝起来像耳垢。

欢迎来到联觉,

一种将 4% 的人口中的两种或多种感觉结合在一起的神经现象。

联觉者不仅可以听到我的声音,

还可以看到

、品尝

或感觉到它是一种身体接触。

与麻醉同根,

即无感觉,

联觉即连有感觉。

拥有一种类型,例如有色听力,

您有 50% 的机会拥有第二种、

第三种

或第四种类型。

在我们当中,每 90 人中就有 1 人体验

过字素,即语言的书面元素,

如字母、

数字

和标点符号

,充满了色彩。

有些甚至有性别或个性。

对盖尔来说,3 是运动型的,

9 是一个自负的精英女孩。

相比之下,语言的声音单位

或音素会

引发联觉。

对詹姆斯来说,大学尝起来像香肠

,信息和

以-age结尾的类似词也是如此。

联觉是一种特征,就像有蓝眼睛一样,

而不是一种疾病,

因为没有错。

事实上,所有额外的钩子都

赋予联觉者卓越的记忆力。

例如,一个女孩遇到了她很久以前认识的人。

“让我们看看,她有一个绿色的名字

。D 是绿色的:

黛布拉、

达比、

多萝西、

丹妮丝。

是的!她的名字是丹妮丝!”

一旦在童年时期建立,

配对就会终生固定。

联觉者继承了大

脑神经元超连接的生物学倾向,

但随后必须接触文化制品,

例如日历、

食物名称

和字母表。

令人惊奇的是,一个

人的 DNA 序列中的单个核苷酸变化会改变感知。

通过这种方式,联觉提供了一种

理解主观差异的途径,

即两个人如何以不同的方式看待同一事物。

以肖恩为例,他喜欢蓝色的食物,

如牛奶、橙子和菠菜。

该基因增强

了他额叶的味觉区域

和更靠后的颜色区域之间正常发生的联系。

但是假设在其他

人中该基因在非感觉区域起作用。

然后,您将有能力将

看似无关的事物联系起来,

这就是隐喻的定义,

在不同之处看到相似之处。

毫不奇怪,联觉

在擅长隐喻的艺术家中更为常见,

如小说家弗拉基米尔·纳博科夫、

画家大卫·霍克尼,

以及作曲家比利·乔尔

和 Lady Gaga。

但是为什么我们这些非联觉者会

理解诸如“锋利的奶酪”

或“甜蜜的人”之类的隐喻呢?

碰巧的是,视觉、

声音

和运动

已经如此紧密地映射在一起,

以至于即使是糟糕的腹语术士也会让我们

相信假人在说话。

同样,电影可以让我们

相信声音来自演员的嘴,

而不是周围的扬声器。

所以,在内心,我们都是联觉者,

外在没有意识到知觉耦合

一直在发生。

大脑中的串扰是规则,

而不是例外。

这对我来说听起来很甜蜜!