The difference between classical and operant conditioning Peggy Andover

Translator: Andrea McDonough
Reviewer: Bedirhan Cinar

When we think about learning,

we often picture students in a classroom or lecture hall,

books open on their desks,

listening intently to a teacher or professor

in the front of the room.

But in psychology, learning means something else.

To psychologists, learning is a long-term change in behavior

that’s based on experience.

Two of the main types of learning are called

classical conditioning

and operant, or instrumental, conditioning.

Let’s talk about classical conditioning first.

In the 1890’s, a Russian physiologist named Ivan Pavlov

did some really famous experiments on dogs.

He showed dogs some food

and rang a bell at the same time.

After a while, the dogs would associate the bell with the food.

They would learn that when they heard the bell,

they would get fed.

Eventually, just ringing the bell

made the dogs salivate.

They learned to expect food at the sound of a bell.

You see, under normal conditions,

the sight and smell of food causes a dog to salivate.

We call the food an unconditioned stimulus,

and we call salivation the unconditioned response.

Nobody trains a dog to salivate over some steak.

However, when we pair an unconditioned stimulus like food

with something that was previously neutral,

like the sound of a bell,

that neutral stimulus becomes a conditioned stimulus.

And so classical conditioning was discovered.

We see how this works with animals,

but how does it work with humans?

In exactly the same way.

Let’s say that one day you go to the doctor to get a shot.

She says, “Don’t worry, this won’t hurt a bit,”

and then gives you the most painful shot you’ve ever had.

A few weeks later you go to the dentist for a check-up.

He starts to put a mirror in your mouth

to examine your teeth,

and he says, “Don’t worry, this won’t hurt a bit.”

Even though you know the mirror won’t hurt,

you jump out of the chair and run,

screaming from the room.

When you went to get a shot,

the words, “This won’t hurt a bit,”

became a conditioned stimulus

when they were paired with pain of the shot,

the unconditioned stimulus,

which was followed by your conditioned response

of getting the heck out of there.

Classical conditioning in action.

Operant conditioning explains how consequences

lead to changes in voluntary behavior.

So how does operant conditioning work?

There are two main components in operant conditioning:

reinforcement and punishment.

Reinforcers make it more likely

that you’ll do something again,

while punishers make it less likely.

Reinforcement and punishment can be positive or negative,

but this doesn’t mean good and bad.

Positive means the addition of a stimulus,

like getting dessert after you finish your veggies,

and negative means the removal of a stimulus,

like getting a night of no homework

because you did well on an exam.

Let’s look at an example of operant conditioning.

After eating dinner with your family,

you clear the table and wash the dishes.

When you’re done, your mom gives you a big hug

and says, “Thank you for helping me.”

In this situation, your mom’s response

is positive reinforcement if it makes you more likely

to repeat the operant response,

which is to clear the table and wash the dishes.

Operant conditioning is everywhere in our daily lives.

There aren’t many things we do

that haven’t been influenced at some point

by operant conditioning.

We even see operant conditioning

in some extraordinary situations.

One group of scientists showed the power

of operant conditioning

by teaching pigeons to be art connoisseurs.

Using food as a positive reinforcer,

scientists have taught pigeons

to select paintings by Monet

over those by Picasso.

When showed works of other artists,

scientists observed stimulus generalization

as the pigeons chose the Impressionists

over the Cubists.

Maybe next they’ll condition the pigeons

to paint their own masterpieces.

译者:Andrea McDonough
审稿人:Bedirhan Cinar

当我们考虑学习时,

我们经常想象学生在教室或报告厅,

课桌上打开书籍,

专心听讲

教室前面的老师或教授。

但在心理学中,学习意味着别的东西。

对心理学家来说,学习是

基于经验的行为的长期变化。

两种主要的学习类型称为

经典条件

反射和操作性或工具性条件反射。

我们先来谈谈经典条件反射。

在 1890 年代,一位名叫伊万·巴甫洛夫的俄罗斯生理学家

在狗身上做了一些非常著名的实验。

他一边给狗看食物

,一边按铃。

过了一会儿,狗会把铃铛和食物联系起来。

他们会知道,当他们听到铃声时,

他们会吃饱。

最终,只要

按铃,狗就会流口水。

他们学会了在铃声响起时期待食物。

你看,在正常情况下,

食物的视觉和气味会导致狗流口水。

我们称食物为无条件刺激

,我们称流涎为无条件反应。

没有人会训练狗在吃牛排时流口水。

然而,当我们将食物等无条件刺激

与以前中性的东西(

如铃声)配对时

,中性刺激就变成了条件刺激。

于是发现了经典条件反射。

我们看到这对动物是如何起作用的,

但它对人类是如何起作用的呢?

以完全相同的方式。

假设有一天你去看医生打针。

她说,“别担心,这不会有一点痛,”

然后给你打了你曾经有过的最痛苦的一针。

几周后,您去看牙医进行检查。

他开始在你嘴里放一面镜子

检查你的牙齿,

然后他说:“别担心,这不会有一点痛。”

即使你知道镜子不会受伤,

你还是从椅子上跳起来跑,

在房间里尖叫。

当你去打针时

,“这不会有一点痛”这句话

变成了条件刺激,

当它们与打针的疼痛相结合时

,无条件的刺激

,然后是你的条件反应

,见鬼 离开那里。

经典条件作用的作用。

操作性条件反射解释了后果如何

导致自愿行为的变化。

那么操作性条件反射是如何工作的呢?

操作性条件反射有两个主要组成部分:

强化和惩罚。

强化者让你更有可能

再次做某事,

而惩罚者则降低它的可能性。

强化和惩罚可以是积极的或消极的,

但这并不意味着好坏。

积极意味着增加刺激,

比如在你吃完蔬菜后得到甜点

,消极意味着取消刺激,

比如因为考试成绩好而没有做作业的晚上

让我们看一个操作性条件反射的例子。

和家人吃完晚饭后,

你收拾桌子,洗碗。

完成后,妈妈给了你一个大大的拥抱,

并说:“谢谢你帮助我。”

在这种情况下,如果你妈妈的反应

让你更有

可能重复操作性反应,

即收拾桌子和洗碗,那么你妈妈的反应就是积极的强化。

操作性条件反射在我们的日常生活中无处不在。

我们所做的很多事情都没有

受到操作性条件反射的影响。

我们甚至会

在一些特殊情况下看到操作性条件反射。

一组科学家

通过教鸽子成为艺术鉴赏家来展示操作性条件反射的力量。

使用食物作为一种积极的强化物,

科学家们教

鸽子选择莫奈的画而

不是毕加索的画。

当展示其他艺术家的作品时,

科学家观察到刺激泛化,

因为鸽子选择印象派

而不是立体派。

也许接下来他们会让

鸽子画出他们自己的杰作。