How conspiracy beliefs reveal intuitive reflective impulses

Transcriber: lisa thompson
Reviewer: David DeRuwe

Is there anything that you believe in
that is implausible?

A Bigfoot, for example?
The afterlife? UFOs?

What about a conspiracy theory?

What all of these
three examples have in common,

aside from the fact that they
are very hard to prove or disprove,

is that they require us
to make a judgment,

a judgment on how likely we think
that our belief is true.

And in doing so, we can either
use our intuition or analytical thinking.

Recent research has shown

that people who believe
in a conspiracy theory

tend to use analytical thinking less
and intuition more.

A conspiracy theory is the belief
that there is a hostile coalition,

a group of people who, in secrecy,
pursue a harmful or deceptive goal.

Conspiracy theories are not wrong, per se,

but they are very often fueled
by misinformation,

and they are hard to disprove.

And remember, some formerly labeled
conspiracy beliefs,

like the Watergate scandal, for example,
turned out to be true later.

Circulating conspiracy beliefs

is a phenomenon that probably
is as old as our humanity itself.

However, the uniqueness of our present age

is that any view, idea, or opinion
can be easily disseminated.

So that is why many of us
may have the feeling

that conspiracy theories
are all around us.

And it is actually not surprising when
you may feel like this even more so lately

because conspiracy theories
often gain momentum in times of crisis.

But why?

When living conditions
have suddenly changed, are threatening,

and when the future seems uncertain,

people tend to experience feelings
like anxiety, stress, frustration,

powerlessness -
all these negative feelings.

And believing in a conspiracy theory

may be one way to minimize
these negative feelings

by assigning meaning to uncertainty.

For instance, take the following example:

It cannot be that the COVID-19 pandemic
happened accidentally.

There must be someone behind it
who did this on purpose

to cause harm to other people,
so it must be man-made.

And the more and more
one feels to be lacking control,

the conspiracy belief becomes
an even more attractive explanation

for otherwise inexplicable events
that have affected us severely.

So interestingly,

believing in a conspiracy theory
does serve a purpose,

namely to establish a sense of control.

Together with my colleagues

from the Universities of Basel,
Oxford, and Lübeck,

we recently discovered
some cognitive characteristics

of people who believe
in a conspiracy theory.

For example, we discovered that they
tend to jump to a conclusion.

And what is really interesting

is that although they find them
rather implausible,

they still choose to believe
in the conclusion they hastily arrived at.

Additionally, we also found out
that once a judgment has been made,

people who believe in a conspiracy theory

tend to not integrate
any further contradictory information

into their judgments anymore.

Importantly - and this is
really remarkable -

not only people who believe
in a conspiracy theory

show these cognitive characteristics;

we all do from time to time.

And that is why, by studying
this unique type of personal judgment,

namely to believe
in a conspiracy theory,

we, as researchers, are learning
about the cognitive underpinnings

of human judgment and decision-making,

and it has taught us
that when making a judgment,

our mind goes through three main cognitive
processes of information gathering:

First, we have to assess
the credibility or plausibility

or accuracy of given information.

Second, we have to decide how much
information we should or want to consult.

And third,

we possibly deal also with information
that contradicts our opinion.

And the way we deal with each
of these three cognitive processes

can tell us whether we are leading
with our intuition or analytical mind.

A simple example is, when choosing
an ice cream flavor,

we may be more led with our intuition,
consulting less information,

not needing to assess the credibility
of different ice cream flavor signs

and do not seek out contradictory views
on favorite ice cream flavors.

But in contrast, when
investing in a big ticket item -

a car, for example, or a flat -

one may wish to gather more information
before purchasing it,

even find negative reviews

and choose specific,
credible sources that we can trust

before making a decision.

The power of intuition is huge,

and I’m pretty sure that all of you
know that positive feeling

when having decided
based on a gut reaction

and the ultimate decision
turned out to be completely right.

Or vice versa -

the negative feeling when you have decided
based on analytical thinking

but the ultimate decision
does not feel right.

We all strive to make
the right decision regardless.

And in general,

all of us use a balance between both:
intuition and analytical thinking.

And all of us need both minds
in order to make judgments and decisions.

We all need both minds

in order to be able to act
and interact with others.

So sometimes we may lean to one side

and make a judgment that requires a leap
to draw a conclusion,

and sometimes we swing
to the other side and overanalyze.

So finally,

believing in a conspiracy theory
in and of itself is not a bad thing.

In the end,

it is just the product of a typical
human process, cognitive process.

It’s what we do with them,
that can be inhuman.

Thank you very much.

(Applause)

抄写员:lisa thompson
审稿人:David DeRuwe

您认为有什么
不可信的吗?

例如,大脚怪?
来世? 不明飞行物?

阴谋论呢?

所有这
三个例子的共同点,

除了
它们很难证明或反驳的事实之外,

是它们要求我们
做出判断

,判断我们
认为我们的信念是真实的可能性有多大。

在这样做的过程中,我们可以
使用我们的直觉或分析思维。

最近的研究表明


相信阴谋论的人

倾向于较少使用分析性思维,
而更多地使用直觉。

阴谋论
相信存在一个敌对的联盟,

一群人秘密地
追求有害或欺骗性的目标。

阴谋论本身并没有错,

但它们经常
受到错误信息的推动,

而且很难反驳。

请记住,一些以前被贴上
阴谋论的标签,

例如水门事件丑闻,
后来证明是真的。

流传的阴谋论

是一种可能
与我们人类本身一样古老的现象。

然而,我们这个时代的独特之处

在于,任何观点、想法或意见
都可以很容易地传播。

这就是为什么我们中的许多人
可能会

觉得阴谋论
无处不在。

事实上,当你最近有这种感觉时,这实际上并不奇怪,

因为阴谋论
经常在危机时期获得动力。

但为什么?

当生活
条件突然发生变化、受到威胁

以及未来似乎不确定时,

人们往往会体验到
焦虑、压力、沮丧、

无能为力等
感觉——所有这些负面情绪。

相信阴谋论

可能是

通过赋予不确定性赋予意义来最小化这些负面情绪的一种方法。

例如,举个例子:

COVID-19 大流行不可能是
偶然发生的。

一定是有人在背后
故意这样

对别人造成伤害,
所以一定是人为的。

越来越多的
人感到缺乏控制

,阴谋信念成为
对严重影响我们

的其他莫名其妙的事件
的更有吸引力的解释。

有趣的是,

相信阴谋论
确实有一个目的,

即建立一种控制感。

我们与

巴塞尔大学、
牛津大学和吕贝克大学的同事一起

,最近发现了
一些

相信阴谋论的人的认知特征

例如,我们发现他们
倾向于草率下结论。

而真正有趣的

是,虽然他们觉得这些结论
相当不可信,

但他们仍然选择相信
自己仓促得出的结论。

此外,我们
还发现,一旦做出判断

,相信阴谋论的人

往往不会再将
任何进一步的矛盾信息

整合到他们的判断中。

重要的是——这
真的很了不起——

不仅
相信阴谋论的人

表现出这些认知特征;

我们都时不时做。

这就是为什么通过研究
这种独特的个人判断,


相信阴谋论,

我们作为研究人员正在学习

人类判断和决策的认知基础

,它告诉我们
,当做出 判断时,

我们的大脑经历
了信息收集的三个主要认知过程:

首先,我们必须评估给定信息
的可信度、合理性

或准确性。

其次,我们必须决定
我们应该或想要查阅多少信息。

第三,

我们也可能处理与
我们的观点相矛盾的信息。

我们
处理这三个认知过程中的每一个的方式都

可以告诉我们,我们是
用我们的直觉还是分析思维来领导。

举个简单的例子,在
选择冰淇淋口味时

,我们可能会更多地被直觉引导,
咨询的信息更少,

不需要评估
不同冰淇淋口味标志的可信度

,也不
会对喜欢的冰淇淋口味寻求矛盾的看法。

但相比之下,在
投资大件物品

时——例如汽车或公寓——

人们可能希望在购买之前收集更多信息
,甚至在购买之前

找到负面评论

并选择
我们可以信任的具体、可靠的来源

决定。

直觉的力量是巨大的

,我很确定你们所有人都
知道


基于直觉

做出决定时的积极感觉,最终的决定
被证明是完全正确的。

反之亦然 -

当您
基于分析性思维

做出决定但最终决定
感觉不正确时的负面感觉。

无论如何,我们都努力
做出正确的决定。

总的来说,

我们所有人都在两者之间取得平衡:
直觉和分析思维。

为了做出判断和决定,我们所有人都需要两种
思想。

我们都需要两种

思想才能行动
并与他人互动。

因此,有时我们可能会偏向一边

并做出需要飞跃
才能得出结论的判断,

而有时我们会
偏向另一边并过度分析。

所以最后,

相信阴谋
论本身并不是一件坏事。

最后,

它只是一个典型的
人类过程,认知过程的产物。

这是我们对他们所做的,
这可能是不人道的。

非常感谢你。

(掌声)