Does grammar matter Andreea S. Calude

You’re telling a friend an amazing story,

and you just get to the best part
when suddenly he interrupts,

“The alien and I,” not “Me and the alien.”

Most of us would probably be annoyed,

but aside from the rude interruption,

does your friend have a point?

Was your sentence actually
grammatically incorrect?

And if he still understood it,
why does it even matter?

From the point of view of linguistics,

grammar is a set of patterns
for how words are put together

to form phrases or clauses,

whether spoken or in writing.

Different languages
have different patterns.

In English, the subject
normally comes first,

followed by the verb,

and then the object,

while in Japanese
and many other languages,

the order is subject, object, verb.

Some scholars have tried to identify
patterns common to all languages,

but apart from some basic features,

like having nouns or verbs,

few of these so-called
linguistic universals have been found.

And while any language needs consistent
patterns to function,

the study of these patterns opens up
an ongoing debate between two positions

known as prescriptivism
and descriptivism.

Grossly simplified,

prescriptivists think a given language
should follow consistent rules,

while descriptivists see variation
and adaptation as a natural

and necessary part of language.

For much of history, the vast majority
of language was spoken.

But as people became more interconnected
and writing gained importance,

written language was standardized
to allow broader communication

and ensure that people in different parts
of a realm could understand each other.

In many languages, this standard form
came to be considered the only proper one,

despite being derived from just one
of many spoken varieties,

usually that of the people in power.

Language purists worked to establish
and propagate this standard

by detailing a set of rules that reflected
the established grammar of their times.

And rules for written grammar were applied
to spoken language, as well.

Speech patterns that deviated from the
written rules were considered corruptions,

or signs of low social status,

and many people who had grown up
speaking in these ways

were forced to adopt
the standardized form.

More recently, however,

linguists have understood that speech
is a separate phenomenon from writing

with its own regularities and patterns.

Most of us learn to speak at such an early
age that we don’t even remember it.

We form our spoken repertoire through
unconscious habits,

not memorized rules.

And because speech also uses mood
and intonation for meaning,

its structure is often more flexible,

adapting to the needs of speakers
and listeners.

This could mean avoiding complex clauses
that are hard to parse in real time,

making changes to avoid awkward
pronounciation,

or removing sounds to make speech faster.

The linguistic approach that tries
to understand and map such differences

without dictating correct ones
is known as descriptivism.

Rather than deciding how language
should be used,

it describes how people actually use it,

and tracks the innovations
they come up with in the process.

But while the debate between

prescriptivism
and descriptivism continues,

the two are not mutually exclusive.

At its best, prescriptivism is useful
for informing people

about the most common established
patterns at a given point in time.

This is important,
not only for formal contexts,

but it also makes communication easier
between non-native speakers

from different backgrounds.

Descriptivism, on the other hand,

gives us insight into how our minds work

and the instinctive ways in which we
structure our view of the world.

Ultimately, grammar is best thought of
as a set of linguistic habits

that are constantly being negotiated
and reinvented

by the entire group of language users.

Like language itself,

it’s a wonderful and complex fabric

woven through the contributions
of speakers and listeners,

writers and readers,

prescriptivists and descriptivists,

from both near and far.

你正在给一个朋友讲一个惊人的故事,

当他突然打断

“外星人和我”而不是“我和外星人”时,你才刚刚到达最好的部分。

我们中的大多数人可能会很生气,

但除了粗鲁的打断之外

,你的朋友有意见吗?

你的句子实际上
语法不正确吗?

如果他仍然理解它,
那这有什么关系呢?

从语言学的角度来看,

语法是一组模式,
用于将单词组合在一起

以形成短语或从句,

无论是口头的还是书面的。

不同的语言
有不同的模式。

在英语中,
通常是主语在前,

然后是动词,

然后是宾语,

而在日语
和许多其他语言中

,顺序是主语、宾语、动词。

一些学者试图找出
所有语言共有的模式,

但除了一些基本特征,

如名词或动词之外,

这些所谓的
语言共性很少被发现。

尽管任何语言都需要一致的
模式才能发挥作用,

但对这些模式的研究
在两种立场之间引发了一场持续的争论,

即规定主义
和描述主义。

大体简化,

规定主义者认为给定的语言
应该遵循一致的规则,

而描述主义者则认为变化
和适应是语言的自然

和必要的部分。

在历史的大部分时间里,
绝大多数语言都是使用的。

但随着人们变得更加相互联系
和写作变得越来越重要,

书面语言被标准化
以允许更广泛的交流

并确保一个领域不同部分
的人们能够相互理解。

在许多语言中,这种标准形式
被认为是唯一合适的形式,

尽管它只是
源自许多口语变体中的一种,

通常是当权者的变体。

语言纯粹主义者

通过详细说明反映
其时代既定语法的一套规则来建立和传播这一标准。

书面语法规则也适用
于口语。

偏离
书面规则的说话方式被认为是腐败,

或社会地位低下的迹象

,许多长大后
以这种方式说话的

人被迫
采用标准化形式。

然而,最近,

语言学家已经明白,言语
是一种独立于书写的现象,

有其自身的规律和模式。

我们中的大多数人在很小的时候就学会了说话,
以至于我们甚至都不记得了。

我们通过
无意识的习惯

而不是死记硬背的规则来形成我们的口语曲目。

而且由于语音也使用语气
和语调来表达意义,

它的结构往往更灵活,

适应说话者
和听众的需要。

这可能意味着避免
难以实时解析的复杂子句,

进行更改以避免
发音尴尬,

或删除声音以加快语音速度。

试图理解和映射这些差异

而不指定正确差异的语言方法
被称为描述主义。

它不是决定
应该如何使用语言,

而是描述人们实际使用它的方式,

并跟踪
他们在此过程中提出的创新。

但是,虽然

规定主义
和描述主义之间的争论仍在继续

,但两者并不相互排斥。

在最好的情况下,规定主义
有助于告知人们

在给定时间点最常见的既定模式。

这很重要,
不仅对于正式的语境,

而且它也使来自不同背景的
非母语人士之间的交流更加容易

另一方面,描述主义

让我们深入了解我们的思想是如何工作的,

以及我们构建世界观的本能方式

归根结底,语法最好被认为
是一组语言习惯

由整个语言用户群体不断协商和重塑。

就像语言本身一样,

它是一种奇妙而复杂的结构,由来自远近

的演讲者和听众、

作家和读者、

规范主义者和描述主义者的贡献编织而成