Do animals have language Michele Bishop

All animals communicate.

Crabs wave their claws at each other
to signal that they’re healthy

and ready to mate.

Cuttlefish use pigmented skin cells
called chromatophores

to create patterns on their skin
that act as camouflage

or warnings to rivals.

Honeybees perform complex dances

to let other bees know the location
and quality of a food source.

All of these animals have impressive
communication systems,

but do they have language?

To answer that question,

we can look at four specific qualities
that are often associated with language:

discreteness,

grammar,

productivity,

and displacement.

Discreteness means that there is a set
of individual units,

such as sounds or words,

that can be combined
to communicate new ideas,

like a set of refrigerator poetry magnets
you can rearrange

to create different phrases.

Grammar provides a system of rules

that tells you how to combine
those individual units.

Productivity is the ability
to use language

to create an infinite number of messages.

And displacement is the ability
to talk about things

that aren’t right in front of you,

such as past, future, or fictional events.

So, does animal communication exhibit
any of these qualities?

For crabs and cuttlefish,
the answer is no.

They don’t combine their signals
in creative ways.

Those signals also don’t have to be
in a grammatical order,

and they only communicate
current conditions,

like, “I am healthy,”
or “I am poisonous.”

But some animals actually do display
some of these properties.

Bees use the moves, angle, duration,
and intensity of their waggle dance

to describe the location and richness
of a food source.

That source is outside the hive,

so they exhibit the property
of displacement.

They share that language trait
with prairie dogs,

which live in towns of thousands,

and are hunted by coyotes, hawks,
badgers, snakes, and humans.

Their alarms calls indicate
the predator’s size, shape, speed,

and, even for human predators,
what the person is wearing

and if he’s carrying a gun.

Great apes, like chimps and gorillas,
are great communicators, too.

Some have even learned
a modified sign language.

A chimpanzee named Washoe
demonstrated discreteness

by combining multiple signs
into original phrases,

like, “Please open. Hurry.”

Coco, a female gorilla who understands
more than 1000 signs,

and around 2000 words of spoken English

referred to a beloved kitten
that had died.

In doing so, she displayed displacement,

though it’s worth noting that the apes
in both of these examples

were using a human communication system,

not one that appeared
naturally in the wild.

There are many other examples
of sophisticated animal communication,

such as in dolphins,

which use whistles to identify age,
location, names, and gender.

They can also understand some grammar

in a gestural language researchers use
to communicate with them.

However, grammar is not seen
in the dolphin’s natural communication.

While these communication systems

may have some of the qualities
of language we’ve identified,

none display all four.

Even Washoe and Coco’s impressive
abilities are still outpaced

by the language skills
of most three-year-old humans.

And animals' topics of conversation
are usually limited.

Bees talk about food,

prairie dogs talk about predators,

and crabs talk about themselves.

Human language stands alone

due to the powerful combination
of grammar and productivity,

on top of discreteness and displacement.

The human brain can take
a finite number of elements

and create an infinite number of messages.

We can craft and understand
complex sentences,

as well as words that have never
been spoken before.

We can use language to communicate
about an endless range of subjects,

talk about imaginary things,

and even lie.

Research continues to reveal more
and more about animal communication.

It may turn out that human language
and animal communication

aren’t entirely different
but exist on a continuum.

After all, we are all animals.

所有的动物都会交流。

螃蟹互相挥动爪子,
表示它们很健康

,可以交配了。

墨鱼使用称为色素细胞的色素细胞

在皮肤上形成图案
,作为伪装

或警告竞争对手。

蜜蜂表演复杂的舞蹈

,让其他蜜蜂知道
食物来源的位置和质量。

所有这些动物都有令人印象深刻的
交流系统,

但它们有语言吗?

为了回答这个问题,

我们可以看看
通常与语言相关的四种特定品质:

离散性、

语法、

生产力

和位移。

离散性意味着有一
组单独的单元,

例如声音或单词

,可以组合
起来传达新的想法,

就像一组冰箱诗歌磁铁
可以重新排列

以创建不同的短语。

语法提供了一个规则系统

,告诉您如何组合
这些单独的单元。

生产力是
使用

语言创建无限数量信息的能力。

而置换是
谈论

不正确的事情的能力,

例如过去、未来或虚构的事件。

那么,动物交流是否表现
出这些品质中的任何一种?

对于螃蟹和墨鱼
,答案是否定的。

他们不会
以创造性的方式组合他们的信号。

这些信号也不必
按语法顺序排列

,它们只传达
当前状况,

例如“我很健康”
或“我有毒”。

但有些动物确实表现出
其中一些特性。

蜜蜂使用摇摆舞的动作、角度、持续时间
和强度

来描述食物来源的位置和丰富
程度。

那个来源在蜂巢之外,

所以它们表现出位移的特性

他们与草原土拨鼠有着共同的语言特征
,草原

土拨鼠生活在成千上万的城镇中,

被土狼、鹰、
獾、蛇和人类猎杀。

他们的警报声会
显示捕食者的大小、形状、速度

,甚至对于人类捕食者来说,也会
显示该人的穿着

以及是否携带枪支。

类人猿,如黑猩猩和大猩猩,
也是很好的沟通者。

有些人甚至学会
了一种改良的手语。

一只名叫 Washoe 的黑猩猩

通过将多个标志组合
成原始短语来展示其离散性,

例如“请打开。快点”。

Coco 是一只能够理解
1000 多个标志

和大约 2000 个英语口语的雌性大猩猩,它

提到了一只已经死去的心爱的
小猫。

在这样做的过程中,她表现出了位移,

但值得注意的是,这
两个例子

中的猿类都使用了人类交流系统,

而不是
自然出现在野外的系统。

还有许多其他
复杂动物交流的例子,

例如海豚,

它们使用口哨来识别年龄、
位置、名字和性别。

他们还可以理解

研究人员用来与他们交流的手势语言中的一些语法

然而,
海豚的自然交流中看不到语法。

虽然这些交流系统

可能具有
我们已经确定的一些语言品质,

但没有一个能同时具备这四种。

即使是 Washoe 和 Coco 令人印象深刻的
能力

,大多数三岁人类的语言能力仍然超过了他们。

动物的谈话话题
通常是有限的。

蜜蜂谈论食物,

土拨鼠谈论掠食者

,螃蟹谈论自己。

由于
语法和生产力的强大结合

,除了离散性和位移之外,人类语言是独立的。

人脑可以
采用有限数量的元素

并创建无限数量的信息。

我们可以制作和理解
复杂的

句子,以及
以前从未说过的单词。

我们可以用语言
交流无穷无尽的话题,

谈论想象的事物,

甚至撒谎。

研究继续揭示
越来越多关于动物交流的信息。

事实证明,人类语言
和动物

交流并非完全不同,
而是存在于一个连续统一体中。

毕竟,我们都是动物。