What happens when you remove the hippocampus Sam Kean

On September 1st, 1953,

William Scoville used a hand crank
and a cheap drill saw

to bore into a young man’s skull,
cutting away vital pieces of his brain

and sucking them out through a metal tube.

But this wasn’t a scene from a horror film
or a gruesome police report.

Dr. Scoville was one of the most
renowned neurosurgeons of his time,

and the young man was Henry Molaison,
the famous patient known as “H.M.”,

whose case provided amazing insights
into how our brains work.

As a boy, Henry had cracked
his skull in an accident

and soon began having seizures, blacking out
and losing control of bodily functions.

After enduring years of frequent episodes,
and even dropping out of high school,

the desperate young man
had turned to Dr. Scoville,

a daredevil known for risky surgeries.

Partial lobotomies had been used
for decades to treat mental patients

based on the notion that
mental functions were strictly localized

to corresponding brain areas.

Having successfully used them
to reduce seizures in psychotics,

Scoville decided to remove
H.M.’s hippocampus,

a part of the limbic system
that was associated with emotion

but whose function was unknown.

At first glance,
the operation had succeeded.

H.M.’s seizures virtually disappeared,
with no change in personality,

and his IQ even improved.

But there was one problem:
His memory was shot.

Besides losing most of his memories
from the previous decade,

H.M. was unable to form new ones,
forgetting what day it was,

repeating comments,
and even eating multiple meals in a row.

When Scoville informed another expert,
Wilder Penfield, of the results,

he sent a Ph.D student named Brenda Milner
to study H.M. at his parents' home,

where he now spent his days
doing odd chores,

and watching classic movies
for the first time, over and over.

What she discovered through
a series of tests and interviews

didn’t just contribute greatly
to the study of memory.

It redefined what memory even meant.

One of Milner’s findings shed light
on the obvious fact

that although H.M. couldn’t form new memories,
he still retained information

long enough from moment to moment
to finish a sentence or find the bathroom.

When Milner gave him a random number,

he managed to remember it
for fifteen minutes

by repeating it to himself constantly.

But only five minutes later,
he forgot the test had even taken place.

Neuroscientists had though of memory
as monolithic,

all of it essentially the same
and stored throughout the brain.

Milner’s results were not only the first
clue for the now familiar distinction

between short-term and long-term memory,

but show that each uses
different brain regions.

We now know that memory formation
involves several steps.

After immediate sensory data is temporarily
transcribed by neurons in the cortex,

it travels to the hippocampus,

where special proteins work to strengthen
the cortical synaptic connections.

If the experience was strong enough,

or we recall it periodically
in the first few days,

the hippocampus then transfers the memory
back to the cortex for permanent storage.

H.M.’s mind could form
the initial impressions,

but without a hippocampus
to perform this memory consolidation,

they eroded,
like messages scrawled in sand.

But this was not the
only memory distinction Milner found.

In a now famous experiment,
she asked H.M. to trace a third star

in the narrow space between
the outlines of two concentric ones

while he could only see
his paper and pencil through a mirror.

Like anyone else performing such
an awkward task for the first time,

he did horribly.

But surprisingly, he improved over
repeated trials,

even though he had no memory
of previous attempts.

His unconscious motor centers remembered
what the conscious mind had forgotten.

What Milner had discovered was that the
declarative memory of names, dates and facts

is different from the procedural memory
of riding a bicycle or signing your name.

And we now know that procedural memory

relies more on the basal ganglia
and cerebellum,

structures that were intact in H.M.’s brain.

This distinction between “knowing that”
and “knowing how”

has underpinned all memory research since.

H.M. died at the age of 82 after
a mostly peaceful life in a nursing home.

Over the years, he had been examined
by more than 100 neuroscientists,

making his the most
studied mind in history.

Upon his death, his brain was
preserved and scanned

before being cut into over 2000
individual slices

and photographed to form a digital map
down to the level of individual neurons,

all in a live broadcast
watched by 400,000 people.

Though H.M. spent most of his life
forgetting things,

he and his contributions
to our understanding of memory

will be remembered for
generations to come.

1953 年 9 月 1 日,

威廉·斯科维尔(William Scoville)使用手摇曲柄
和廉价

钻锯钻入一名年轻人的头骨,
切掉他大脑的重要部分,

并通过金属管将其吸出。

但这不是恐怖电影中的场景,
也不是可怕的警察报告。

斯科维尔
医生是他那个时代最著名的神经外科医生之一

,这位年轻人就是
著名的病人亨利莫莱森,

他的案例
为我们的大脑如何工作提供了惊人的见解。

作为一个男孩,亨利
在一次事故中颅骨

骨折,很快就开始癫痫发作、昏迷
并失去对身体机能的控制。

在忍受了多年的频繁发作,
甚至从高中辍学之后,

这个绝望的
年轻人转向了斯科维尔博士,他

是一个以冒险手术而闻名的冒险者。 几十年来,

部分脑叶切除术一直
用于治疗精神病患者,其

依据是
心理功能严格定位

于相应的大脑区域。

在成功地使用它们
来减少精神病患者的癫痫发作后,

斯科维尔决定移除
H.M. 的海马体,

这是与情绪相关

但功能未知的边缘系统的一部分。

乍一看
,手术成功了。

H.M.的癫痫发作几乎消失了
,性格没有任何变化

,他的智商甚至有所提高。

但是有一个问题:
他的记忆被击中了。

除了失去
过去十年的大部分记忆外,

H.M. 无法形成新的,
忘记今天是什么日子,

重复评论,
甚至连续吃多顿饭。

当斯科维尔将结果告知另一位专家
怀尔德彭菲尔德时,

他派了一位名叫布伦达米尔纳的博士生
来研究 H.M. 在他父母的家里

,他现在每天都在那里
做一些零碎的家务,


第一次一遍又一遍地看经典电影。

她通过
一系列测试和采访发现的东西

不仅
对记忆研究有很大贡献。

它重新定义了记忆的含义。

米尔纳的一项发现
揭示了一个明显的事实

,即尽管 H.M. 无法形成新的记忆,
他仍然

时不时地保留足够长的信息,
以完成一句话或找到洗手间。

当米尔纳给他一个随机数时,

通过不断地对自己重复,设法记住了十五分钟。

但仅仅过了五分钟,
他就忘记了测试已经发生了。

神经科学家认为记忆
是整体的,

所有这些都基本相同,
并存储在整个大脑中。

米尔纳的研究结果不仅
为现在熟悉

的短期记忆和长期记忆之间的区别提供了第一条线索,

而且表明每个人都使用
不同的大脑区域。

我们现在知道记忆的形成
涉及几个步骤。

在直接的感觉数据
被皮层中的神经元暂时转录后,

它会传播到海马体,

在那里特殊的蛋白质会起到
加强皮层突触连接的作用。

如果体验足够强烈,

或者我们在最初几天定期回忆它

,海马体就会将记忆转移
回皮层进行永久存储。

H.M. 的头脑可以
形成最初的印象,

但没有海马体
来执行这种记忆巩固,

它们就会被侵蚀,
就像在沙子里潦草的信息一样。

但这并不是
米尔纳发现的唯一记忆区别。

在一个现在著名的实验中,
她问 H.M.

在两个同心圆的轮廓之间的狭窄空间中追踪第三颗星星,

而他只能
通过镜子看到他的纸和铅笔。

像其他
第一次执行如此尴尬的任务的人一样,

他做得很糟糕。

但令人惊讶的是,

尽管他不
记得以前的尝试,但他在反复试验中有所改善。

他的无意识运动中心记得
有意识的头脑忘记了什么。

米尔纳发现,
对姓名、日期和事实的陈述

性记忆不同于
骑自行车或签名的程序性记忆。

而且我们现在知道,程序性记忆

更多地依赖于基底神经节
和小脑,这些

结构在 H.M. 的大脑中是完整的。 从那时起

,“知道”和“知道如何”之间的这种区别

一直是所有记忆研究的基础。

H.M.
在疗养院过着平静的生活后,享年 82 岁。

多年来,他接受了
100 多名神经科学家的检查,

使他成为
历史上研究最多的大脑。

在他去世后,他的大脑被
保存并扫描,

然后被切割成 2000 多个
单独的切片

并拍照以形成一个
低至单个神经元水平的数字地图,

所有这些都在
400,000 人观看的现场直播中。

虽然 H.M. 他一生大部分时间都在
遗忘事物,

他和他
对我们理解记忆的贡献

将被
后代铭记。