How can you change someones mind hint facts arent always enough Hugo Mercier

Three people are at a dinner party.

Paul, who’s married, is looking at Linda.

Meanwhile, Linda is looking at John,
who’s not married.

Is someone who’s married looking
at someone who’s not married?

Take a moment to think about it.

Most people answer that there’s
not enough information to tell.

And most people are wrong.

Linda must be either married
or not married—there are no other options.

So in either scenario, someone married
is looking at someone who’s not married.

When presented with the explanation,
most people change their minds

and accept the correct answer,

despite being very confident
in their first responses.

Now let’s look at another case.

A 2005 study by Brendan Nyhan
and Jason Reifler

examined American attitudes regarding
the justifications for the Iraq War.

Researchers presented participants
with a news article

that showed no weapons
of mass destruction had been found.

Yet many participants not only continued
to believe that WMDs had been found,

but they even became more convinced
of their original views.

So why do arguments change people’s minds
in some cases and backfire in others?

Arguments are more convincing when they
rest on a good knowledge of the audience,

taking into account
what the audience believes,

who they trust,

and what they value.

Mathematical and logical arguments
like the dinner party brainteaser work

because even when people
reach different conclusions,

they’re starting from
the same set of shared beliefs.

In 1931, a young, unknown mathematician
named Kurt Gödel presented a proof

that a logically complete
system of mathematics was impossible.

Despite upending decades of
work by brilliant mathematicians

like Bertrand Russell and David Hilbert,

the proof was accepted

because it relied on axioms that
everyone in the field already agreed on.

Of course, many disagreements involve
different beliefs

that can’t simply be reconciled
through logic.

When these beliefs involve
outside information,

the issue often comes down to
what sources and authorities people trust.

One study asked people to estimate
several statistics

related to the scope of climate change.

Participants were asked questions,

such as “how many of the years
between 1995 and 2006

were one of the hottest 12 years
since 1850?”

After providing their answers,

they were presented with data from the
Intergovernmental Panel on Climate Change,

in this case showing that the answer
was 11 of the 12 years.

Being provided with these reliable
statistics from a trusted official source

made people more likely to accept
the reality that the earth is warming.

Finally, for disagreements that
can’t be definitively settled

with statistics or evidence,

making a convincing argument

may depend on engaging
the audience’s values.

For example, researchers have conducted
a number of studies

where they’ve asked people
of different political backgrounds

to rank their values.

Liberals in these studies,
on average, rank fairness—

here meaning whether everyone is treated
in the same way—above loyalty.

In later studies, researchers attempted
to convince liberals

to support military spending with
a variety of arguments.

Arguments based on fairness—

like that the military provides employment

and education to people
from disadvantaged backgrounds—

were more convincing than arguments
based on loyalty—

such as that the military
unifies a nation.

These three elements—

beliefs, trusted sources, and values—

may seem like a simple formula
for finding agreement and consensus.

The problem is that our initial
inclination is to think of arguments

that rely on our own beliefs,
trusted sources, and values.

And even when we don’t,

it can be challenging to correctly
identify what’s held dear

by people who don’t already agree with us.

The best way to find out
is simply to talk to them.

In the course of discussion,

you’ll be exposed to counter-arguments
and rebuttals.

These can help you make your own
arguments and reasoning more convincing

and sometimes, you may even end up
being the one changing your mind.

三个人正在参加晚宴。

已婚的保罗看着琳达。

与此同时,琳达看着
未婚的约翰。

已婚
的人会看未婚的人吗?

花点时间考虑一下。

大多数人回答说
没有足够的信息可以告诉。

大多数人都错了。

琳达要么结婚
要么不结婚——别无选择。

所以在任何一种情况下,一个
已婚的人都在看一个未婚的人。

当看到解释时,
大多数人会改变主意

并接受正确的答案,

尽管他们
对他们的第一反应非常有信心。

现在让我们看另一个案例。

Brendan Nyhan 和 Jason Reifler 于 2005 年进行的一项研究调查

了美国人
对伊拉克战争正当性的态度。

研究人员向参与者展示
了一篇新闻文章

,显示
没有发现大规模杀伤性武器。

然而,许多参与者不仅
继续相信已经发现了大规模杀伤性武器,

而且他们甚至更加
相信他们最初的观点。

那么为什么争论
在某些情况下会改变人们的想法,而在其他情况下会适得其反呢?

当论点
建立在对听众的充分了解的基础上,

考虑
到听众的信仰、

他们信任的人

以及他们看重的东西时,争论就会更有说服力。

诸如晚宴脑筋急转弯之类的数学和逻辑论证之所以有效,

是因为即使人们
得出不同的结论,

他们也是
从相同的共同信念开始的。

1931 年,一位
名叫库尔特·哥德尔 (Kurt Gödel) 的年轻无名数学家提出了一个证明

,证明一个逻辑上完整
的数学系统是不可能的。

尽管颠覆了

伯特兰·罗素(Bertrand Russell)和大卫·希尔伯特(David Hilbert)等杰出数学家几十年的工作,但

这个证明被接受了,

因为它依赖
于该领域每个人都已经同意的公理。

当然,许多分歧涉及
不同的信念

,不能简单地
通过逻辑来调和。

当这些信念涉及
外部信息时

,问题通常归结为
人们信任的来源和权威。

一项研究要求人们估计

与气候变化范围有关的几个统计数据。

参与者被问到一些问题,

例如“
1995 年至 2006 年

间有多少年是自 1850 年以来最热的 12 年之一
?”

在提供答案后,

他们收到了来自
政府间气候变化专门委员会的数据,

在这种情况下显示答案
是 12 年中的 11 年。

从可信赖的官方来源获得这些可靠的统计数据

使人们更有可能接受
地球正在变暖的现实。

最后,对于
无法

通过统计数据或证据明确解决的分歧,

提出令人信服的论点

可能取决于
吸引听众的价值观。

例如,研究人员进行
了许多研究

,他们要求
不同政治背景的人

对他们的价值观进行排名。 平均而言,

这些研究中的自由主义者
将公平——

这里的意思是是否每个人都
受到同等对待——高于忠诚度。

在后来的研究中,研究人员试图

用各种论据说服自由主义者支持军费开支

基于公平的论点——

比如军队为处境不利的人提供就业

和教育——


基于忠诚的论点——

比如军队
统一一个国家——更有说服力。

这三个要素——

信念、可信来源和价值观——

似乎是一个
寻找共识和共识的简单公式。

问题在于,我们最初的
倾向是考虑

依赖于我们自己的信仰、
可信来源和价值观的论点。

即使我们不这样做,

正确
识别

那些尚未同意我们的人所珍视的东西也可能具有挑战性。

找出答案的最好方法
就是与他们交谈。

在讨论过程中,

您将面临
反驳和反驳。

这些可以帮助您使自己的
论点和推理更有说服力

,有时,您甚至可能最终
成为改变主意的人。