Why are some people lefthanded Daniel M. Abrams

If you know an older left-handed person,

chances are they had to learn to write
or eat with their right hand.

And in many parts of the world,

it’s still common practice to force
children to use their “proper” hand.

Even the word for right
also means correct or good,

not just in English,
but many other languages, too.

But if being left-handed is so wrong,

then why does it happen
in the first place?

Today, about 1/10 of the world’s
population are left-handed.

Archeological evidence shows
that it’s been that way

for as long as 500,000 years,

with about 10% of human remains

showing the associated differences
in arm length and bone density,

and some ancient tools and artifacts
showing evidence of left-hand use.

And despite what many may think,
handedness is not a choice.

It can be predicted even before birth
based on the fetus' position in the womb.

So, if handedness is inborn,
does that mean it’s genetic?

Well, yes and no.

Identical twins, who have the same genes,
can have different dominant hands.

In fact, this happens as often as it does
with any other sibling pair.

But the chances of being
right or left-handed

are determined by the handedness
of your parents

in surprisingly consistent ratios.

If your father was left-handed
but your mother was right-handed,

you have a 17% chance
of being born left-handed,

while two righties will have
a left-handed child only 10% of the time.

Handedness seems to be determined
by a roll of the dice,

but the odds are set by your genes.

All of this implies there’s a reason

that evolution has produced
this small proportion of lefties,

and maintained it
over the course of millennia.

And while there have been several theories

attempting to explain why handedness
exists in the first place,

or why most people are right-handed,

a recent mathematical model

suggests that the actual ratio
reflects a balance

between competitive and cooperative
pressures on human evolution.

The benefits of being left-handed

are clearest in activities
involving an opponent,

like combat or competitive sports.

For example, about 50% of top hitters
in baseball have been left-handed.

Why?

Think of it as a surprise advantage.

Because lefties are a minority
to begin with,

both right-handed
and left-handed competitors

will spend most of their time
encountering

and practicing against righties.

So when the two face each other,

the left-hander will be better prepared
against this right-handed opponent,

while the righty will be thrown off.

This fighting hypothesis,

where an imbalance in the population

results in an advantage for left-handed
fighters or athletes,

is an example of negative
frequency-dependent selection.

But according to the principles
of evolution,

groups that have a relative advantage

tend to grow until
that advantage disappears.

If people were only fighting and competing
throughout human evolution,

natural selection would lead to more
lefties being the ones that made it

until there were so many of them,

that it was no longer a rare asset.

So in a purely competitive world,

50% of the population
would be left-handed.

But human evolution has been shaped
by cooperation, as well as competition.

And cooperative pressure

pushes handedness distribution
in the opposite direction.

In golf, where performance
doesn’t depend on the opponent,

only 4% of top players are left-handed,

an example of the wider phenomenon
of tool sharing.

Just as young potential golfers

can more easily find
a set of right-handed clubs,

many of the important instruments
that have shaped society

were designed for
the right-handed majority.

Because lefties are worse
at using these tools,

and suffer from higher accident rates,

they would be less successful
in a purely cooperative world,

eventually disappearing
from the population.

So by correctly predicting
the distribution

of left-handed people
in the general population,

as well as matching data
from various sports,

the model indicates

that the persistence of lefties
as a small but stable minority

reflects an equilibrium

that comes from competitive
and cooperative effects

playing out simultaneously over time.

And the most intriguing thing

is what the numbers can tell us
about various populations.

From the skewed distribution of pawedness
in cooperative animals,

to the slightly larger
percentage of lefties

in competitive hunter-gatherer societies,

we may even find that the answers
to some puzzles of early human evolution

are already in our hands.

如果你认识一个年长的左撇子,

他们很可能不得不学会
用右手写字或吃饭。

在世界许多地方

,强迫
孩子使用他们“正确”的手仍然是常见的做法。

即使是正确的词
也意味着正确或良好,

不仅在英语中,
而且在许多其他语言中也是如此。

但是,如果左撇子是错误的,

那么为什么会发生这种
情况呢?

今天,世界上大约 1/10 的
人口是左撇子。

考古证据表明
,这种方式已经存在

了 500,000 年之久

,大约 10% 的人类遗骸

显示
出臂长和骨密度的相关差异

,一些古代工具和文物
显示出左手使用的证据。

尽管许多人可能会这么想,但
惯用手并不是一种选择。

它甚至可以在出生前
根据胎儿在子宫中的位置进行预测。

那么,如果惯用手是天生的,
这是否意味着它是遗传的?

嗯,是的,也不是。

具有相同基因的同卵双胞胎
可以拥有不同的优势手。

事实上,这种情况
与任何其他兄弟姐妹一样经常发生。

但是
左撇子或右撇子的几率

是由你父母的惯用手决定的,

比例惊人地一致。

如果你的父亲是左撇子,
而你的母亲是右撇子,

那么你有 17% 的
机会是左撇子,

而两个
右撇子的孩子只有 10% 的机会是左撇子。

惯用手似乎是
由掷骰子决定的,

但几率是由你的基因决定的。

所有这一切都

意味着进化产生了
这么少比例的左撇子,


在几千年的过程中保持这种状态是有原因的。

虽然有几种理论

试图解释为什么一开始就存在惯用手

或者为什么大多数人都是右撇子

,但最近的一个数学模型

表明,实际比率
反映

了人类进化中竞争压力和合作压力之间的平衡。

左撇子的好处

在涉及对手的活动中最为明显

例如格斗或竞技运动。

例如,大约 50% 的棒球顶级击球手
是左撇子。

为什么?

把它想象成一个惊喜的优势。

因为左撇子一
开始是少数,

所以左撇子
和右撇子的选手

都会花费大部分时间来

对抗和练习右撇子。

所以当两人面对面时

,左撇子会更好地
对抗这个右撇子的对手,而右撇子

则会被甩掉。

这种格斗假设,

即人口不平衡

导致左撇子
拳手或运动员有优势,

是负
频率依赖选择的一个例子。

但是根据
进化原理

,具有相对优势的群体

往往会增长,直到
这种优势消失。

如果人们在整个人类进化过程中只是在战斗和竞争

自然选择会导致更多
的左撇子成为左撇子,

直到他们数量如此之多,

以至于它不再是一种稀有资产。

因此,在一个纯粹竞争的世界中,

50% 的人口
将是左撇子。

但人类进化是
由合作和竞争塑造的。

合作压力

将左右手分布
推向相反的方向。

在高尔夫中,表现
不取决于对手,

只有 4% 的顶级球员是左撇子,

这是更广泛
的工具共享现象的一个例子。

正如年轻的潜在高尔夫球手

可以更容易地找到
一套右撇子球杆一样,

许多塑造社会的重要工具

都是
为大多数右撇子设计的。

因为左撇子更不
擅长使用这些工具,

并且遭受更高的事故率,

所以他们
在纯粹合作的世界中不太成功,

最终
从人群中消失。

因此,通过正确预测

左撇子
在普通人群中的分布,

以及匹配
来自各种运动的数据,

该模型

表明,左撇子
作为一个小而稳定的少数群体的持续存在

反映了

一种来自竞争
和合作效应的平衡。

随着时间的推移同时出来。

最有趣的

是这些数字可以告诉我们
关于不同人群的信息。

从合作动物的爪子分布
偏斜,

竞争激烈的狩猎采集社会中左撇子比例略高,

我们甚至可能会发现,
早期人类进化的一些谜题的答案

已经掌握在我们手中。