Why incompetent people think theyre amazing David Dunning

Are you as good at things
as you think you are?

How good are you at managing money?

What about reading people’s emotions?

How healthy are you
compared to other people you know?

Are you better than average at grammar?

Knowing how competent we are

and how are skill stack up
against other people’s

is more than a self-esteem boost.

It helps us figure out when we can forge
ahead on our own decisions and instincts

and when we need, instead,
to seek out advice.

But psychological research suggests
that we’re not very good

at evaluating ourselves accurately.

In fact, we frequently overestimate
our own abilities.

Researchers have a name
for this phenomena,

the Dunning-Kruger effect.

This effect explains
why more than 100 studies

have shown that people display
illusory superiority.

We judge ourselves as better than others

to a degree that violates
the laws of math.

When software engineers at two companies
were asked to rate their performance,

32% of the engineers at one company
and 42% at the other

put themselves in the top 5%.

In another study, 88% of American drivers

described themselves
as having above average driving skills.

These aren’t isolated findings.

On average, people tend to rate
themselves better than most

in disciplines ranging from health,
leadership skills, ethics, and beyond.

What’s particularly interesting
is that those with the least ability

are often the most likely to overrate
their skills to the greatest extent.

People measurably poor
at logical reasoning,

grammar,

financial knowledge,

math,

emotional intelligence,

running medical lab tests,

and chess

all tend to rate their expertise almost
as favorably as actual experts do.

So who’s most vulnerable to this delusion?

Sadly, all of us because we all have
pockets of incompetence

we don’t recognize.

But why?

When psychologists Dunning and Kruger
first described the effect in 1999,

they argued that people lacking
knowledge and skill in particular areas

suffer a double curse.

First, they make mistakes
and reach poor decisions.

But second, those same knowledge gaps also
prevent them from catching their errors.

In other words, poor performers lack
the very expertise needed

to recognize how badly they’re doing.

For example, when the researchers studied

participants in
a college debate tournament,

the bottom 25% of teams
in preliminary rounds

lost nearly four
out of every five matches.

But they thought they were winning
almost 60%.

WIthout a strong grasp
of the rules of debate,

the students simply couldn’t recognize
when or how often

their arguments broke down.

The Dunning-Kruger effect isn’t a question
of ego blinding us to our weaknesses.

People usually do admit their deficits
once they can spot them.

In one study, students who had initially
done badly on a logic quiz

and then took a mini course on logic

were quite willing to label
their original performances as awful.

That may be why people with a moderate
amount of experience or expertise

often have less confidence
in their abilities.

They know enough to know that
there’s a lot they don’t know.

Meanwhile, experts tend to be aware
of just how knowledgeable they are.

But they often make a different mistake:

they assume that everyone else
is knowledgeable, too.

The result is that people,
whether they’re inept or highly skilled,

are often caught in a bubble
of inaccurate self-perception.

When they’re unskilled,
they can’t see their own faults.

When they’re exceptionally competent,

they don’t perceive how unusual
their abilities are.

So if the Dunning-Kruger effect
is invisible to those experiencing it,

what can you do to find out how good
you actually are at various things?

First, ask for feedback from other people,

and consider it,
even if it’s hard to hear.

Second, and more important, keep learning.

The more knowledgeable we become,

the less likely we are to have
invisible holes in our competence.

Perhaps it all boils down
to that old proverb:

When arguing with a fool,

first make sure the other person
isn’t doing the same thing.

你是否像你认为的那样擅长事情?

你在理财方面有多好?

读懂人的情绪怎么样?

与您认识的其他人相比,您的健康状况如何?

你的语法比平均水平好吗?

了解我们的能力

以及
与其他人相比的技能

水平不仅仅是提升自尊心。

它可以帮助我们弄清楚我们何时可以
根据自己的决定和直觉进取

,以及何时
需要寻求建议。

但心理学研究表明
,我们并不

擅长准确地评估自己。

事实上,我们经常高估
自己的能力。

研究人员
为这种现象起了一个名字,

即邓宁-克鲁格效应。

这种效应解释了
为什么 100 多项研究

表明人们表现出
虚幻的优越感。

我们认为自己比别人好


违反数学定律的程度。

当要求两家公司的软件工程师
评估他们的表现时,

一家公司 32% 的工程师和另一家
公司 42% 的工程师

将自己排在前 5%。

在另一项研究中,88% 的美国司机


自己的驾驶技能高于平均水平。

这些不是孤立的发现。

平均而言,

人们在健康、
领导技能、道德等方面的评价往往高于大多数人。

特别有趣的
是,能力最差的

人往往最有可能
在最大程度上高估自己的技能。

在逻辑推理、

语法、

金融知识、

数学、

情商、

进行医学实验室测试

和国际象棋方面明显欠佳的人

都倾向于对他们的专业知识进行评价,几乎
与真正的专家一样。

那么谁最容易受到这种错觉的影响呢?

可悲的是,我们所有人,因为我们都有

我们不认识的无能口袋。

但为什么?

当心理学家邓宁和克鲁格
在 1999 年首次描述这种影响时,

他们认为
在特定领域缺乏知识和技能的人

会遭受双重诅咒。

首先,他们会犯错误并做出错误的
决定。

但其次,这些相同的知识差距也
阻止了他们发现错误。

换句话说,表现不佳的人缺乏

识别他们做得有多糟糕所需的专业知识。

例如,当研究人员研究

大学辩论锦标赛的参与者时,预赛中排名

垫底的 25% 的球队

每五场比赛中输掉了近四场。

但他们认为他们赢了
将近 60%。

如果没有很好地
掌握辩论规则

,学生们根本无法识别

他们的论点何时或多久失败。

邓宁-克鲁格效应
不是自我让我们看不到自己的弱点的问题。

人们通常会在发现缺陷后承认自己的缺陷

在一项研究中,最初
在逻辑测验中表现不佳

然后参加逻辑迷你课程的

学生非常愿意将
他们最初的表现标记为糟糕。

这可能就是为什么具有
中等经验或专业知识的人

通常对
自己的能力缺乏信心的原因。

他们知道的足够多,知道
还有很多他们不知道。

与此同时,专家往往会意识到
他们的知识渊博。

但他们经常犯一个不同的错误:

他们认为其他人
也是知识渊博的。

结果是人们,
无论他们是无能还是高技能,

经常陷入
自我认知不准确的泡沫中。

当他们不熟练时,
他们看不到自己的缺点。

当他们非常有能力时,

他们不会意识到
他们的能力有多么不寻常。

因此,如果邓宁-克鲁格效应
对那些体验它的人来说是不可见的,

那么你能做些什么来发现
你在各种事情上的实际表现如何?

首先,征求其他人的反馈,

并考虑它,
即使很难听到。

其次,更重要的是,不断学习。

我们变得知识越多,我们

就越不可能
在我们的能力中出现无形的漏洞。

也许这一切都归结
为那句古老的谚语:

与傻瓜争论时,

首先要确保
对方没有做同样的事情。