The science doesnt lie Cognitive contamination in forensics

Transcriber: Paulina Kaniewska
Reviewer: Rhonda Jacobs

When I was pregnant with my son,
I was diagnosed with gestational diabetes,

and that meant
a lot of extra tests and scans,

and checking my blood sugars
four times a day.

But the funny thing was

every time I got a new test result,
or a data point, if you like,

it seemed to show
that I wasn’t a diabetic.

But when I raised this
with the doctor at the hospital,

he was adamant that I was.

A few months went by, and I asked
for another diagnostic test,

and it was negative.

So I went to the hospital
and asked to speak with the doctor.

And I’m sitting in the doctor’s office,
waiting for him to come in,

bracing myself.

If you’ve ever watched
a modern day murder mystery,

you’ll know that forensic scientists
go to great lengths

to prevent their evidence
becoming contaminated.

You’ll have seen them wearing the suits,
and the booties, and the gloves.

And we wear those things
to stop our physical evidence,

things like DNA swabs,
fingerprints, shoe marks,

coming into contact with people or things
that aren’t relevant to the case.

Now, I’m a forensic scientist,

but today I want to talk about
a totally different type of contamination,

and that’s cognitive contamination.

And what that means
is how our scientific thinking

can become contaminated
by details about the case that we learn.

Of course, we all love to think
that we’re purely objective,

and we always make
a 100 percent scientific decision.

But sometimes things
that have nothing to do do with science

can creep in to our decision-making,

and we’re not even aware
that it’s happened.

My area of expertise
is forensic toxicology,

and that means I’m interested
in drugs and alcohol,

well, professionally interested
in drugs and alcohol,

and the role they play
in deaths and crimes.

And a really common type of case
a forensic toxicologist might deal with

is driving under the influence of drugs.

In those cases, typically, we’re sent
a single blood sample from a driver

and asked to test it.

So one day one of these samples
arrives in my lab,

and the police officer in charge
of the case phones me up and says:

“We’ve already caught
this 21-year-old male driver nine times

for driving around on drugs,

so we’re pretty sure he’s guilty,

and the previous nine times
he’d taken methamphetamine,

which you may know as crystal meth.

So the first step in any tox case
is to decide which test to do.

Despite what you might have seen on CSI,

there is no one amazing “magic machine”

that can test for every drug
and poison ever.

There is usually some element
of picking and choosing a test,

and we want to choose the test
that will be most useful for the case.

So, which test do you think
would be most useful on this case?

Well, some of you
might be sitting there thinking:

“Your job sounds pretty easy.
Just test the blood for methamphetamine.”

And that is the most obvious decision.

But it’s also a biased decision

because it’s been influenced
by what I’ve been told

about the previous behavior of the driver.

And we call this
“expected frequency bias.”

And it’s the reason jurors are not told
about previous convictions.

But even if I know nothing

about the previous behavior
of the driver,

just knowing a few personal details

can lead to the same kind
of biased thinking.

Toxicologists will sometimes choose
tests based on assumptions

about the sorts of people who use drugs.

Assumptions based on age, gender,
ethnicity, even sexuality.

And the problem with these assumptions
is that they are just that,

they are just guesses,

and they’re very often not based
on anything particularly scientific.

And the danger in using them

is that it’s really easy
to miss something.

I could just look for
methamphetamine in this case.

But what if that result is negative?

What if the driver’s taken
something else, and I miss it

because I don’t look properly,

because I’ve been swayed
by what the police have told me.

So I need to be more open-minded,
and I decide to do a screening test

that, yes, can see methamphetamine,

but can also see other drugs,
like cannabis and cocaine.

A few days later,
I get my test results back,

and they’re positive for methamphetamine.

“Surprise, surprise,”
you might be thinking,

but remember I said
it was a screening test.

What that means is that all I have
is a simple “yes” or “no,”

and the result is preliminary.

It’s very like
a lateral flow test for COVID-19.

The negatives are OK, but the positives
need to be confirmed by another method.

In forensic toxicology,
we call that method “mass spectrometry.”

And just like your PCR test for COVID-19,

it takes longer to do
and it’s more expensive.

And it also gives us
a more complex answer.

So instead of that simple “yes” or “no,”

we get a picture,
a picture on a computer screen,

and it’s my job as a toxicologist
to compare that picture to another one

and do a sort of a “spot the difference.”

So let’s look at our first picture,
and we call this “a mass spectrum.”

Now, this is a picture
of methamphetamine powder,

something I know
is definitely methamphetamine.

You can see in the picture
we have three vertical lines,

and each of those lines
represents a number.

On the left - 91, in the middle - 119,
on the right - 150.

Those three numbers together
tell me that this is methamphetamine.

There is another important detail,

and that is how tall each of the lines
is compared to each other.

You can see that the tallest is actually
the one in the middle, the 119.

And the next tallest
is the one on the right, 150.

Shortest is the 91.

Now, let’s look at our case.

Hopefully, you can see straight away
that there are some extra lines in here

that are not in the methamphetamine.

So we’ve got a really big one there at 88,
another one at 60, and so on.

What do those lines mean?
Are they important?

Are they just some rubbish
that I can ignore?

If you’re eagle-eyed,
you will have also seen

the ratio between the lines isn’t right,

but that 150 out there on the right -
that is shorter than it should be.

But yet, I do have my three numbers.

And this is the problem
with real case samples.

They’re messy and hard to interpret.

And this is what we call
in technical terms “an iffy match.”

And when results are iffy,

that’s when that case information
can start to creep in.

I think that this sample comes from

a 21-year-old male driver
who’s already been caught

using methamphetamine nine times.

And knowing that
I might subconsciously upgrade this

from “iffy” to “definitely positive.”

“Yes, OK, there are some
extra lines in there,

but they’re probably
just some rubbish that I can ignore,

and I have all three numbers.

I’m going to say it’s positive.”

Now, what if I think this is being taken
from a 92-year-old grandma in a rest home?

I might subconsciously downgrade this
from “iffy” to “definitely negative.”

“What about extra lines at the start?
I really don’t think I can ignore those.

And the ratio between the lines
is definitely off.

I’m going to say it’s negative.”

And if I did either of those two things,
it would be called “contextual bias”.

The fact is that when the exact same data
is interpreted as positive

for a 21-year-old male
but negative for a 92-year-old female,

that’s a real problem
for the criminal justice system.

It’s also important to know

that not all of the information
I get is correct.

People don’t always tell the police
the truth in the aftermath of a crime.

Some of the stuff I’m told
is basically just hearsay

that would never be allowed in court,

but yet it’s allowed to influence
my scientific decision-making.

So I phone the police officer,
and I give him the news.

“I can’t be sure

there is methamphetamine
in this sample.”

Now, that doesn’t mean it isn’t there.
It doesn’t mean the driver didn’t do it.

What it means is there isn’t
enough scientific proof to say

there is methamphetaminein the sample.

That might not seem to you
like the best outcome

for the police or the prosecution,

but it’s the one that’s based on science,

not the one that’s based on
what I’ve been told about the driver.

In the title of my talk,
I mention a battle,

and there are many battles going on
in forensic science right now

over these very issues.

There’s a particularly fierce one
raging in forensic pathology right now.

And the pathologist works very closely
with the toxicologist

because it’s their job

to work out the cause
and sometimes the manner of death.

But in my field,
colleagues will say to me:

“We use machines to get our data.
And machines are objective, right?

So how could our results be biased?”

But as you’ve seen today, the problem
exists between the keyboard and the chair.

Yes, machines give us objective data,

but it’s we humans with all our biases
who decide what that data means.

My colleagues will also say to me:

“I’ll just forget what I’ve been told.

I just won’t use that when I come
to make a scientific decision.”

Sadly, we just don’t have that kind
of level of control over our subconscious.

So let’s go back to that doctor’s office

where I’m sitting, waiting,
heavily pregnant.

On the one hand,

I have a single positive
result for diabetes;

on the other, I have a mountain
of negative data.

Then something
I was not expecting happened,

and a new doctor walked into the room.

And she looked at the data,
and then she looked at me and said:

“This is a misdiagnosis.
You don’t have gestational diabetes.”

And I was so relieved,
but it got me thinking.

“What was happening here?

Why were these two medical professionals
looking at the same data

and coming to opposite conclusions?”

Or, to put it another way:

“What was stopping the first doctor
seeing the mountain of negative data?”

And what was stopping him
was that he knew something about me

that the second doctor didn’t.

He knew that diabetes runs in my family.

And because he knew that,
he couldn’t forget it.

And he was convinced that because others
in my family have had diabetes,

I must have it too.

And this is called “confirmation bias,”

and it’s the last piece
of the cognitive bias puzzle.

Because once we learn something
about a patient or a case,

we can’t unlearn it,

and we start to look for data or evidence

that is consistent with the thing
that we’ve learned.

And we start to ignore evidence
that is not consistent

with the thing that we’ve learned.

So I want to leave you
with a question now.

Given what we know

about how cognitive contamination
can affect forensic toxicology,

isn’t it about time we started
protecting our evidence from it?

Thank you.

(Applause)

抄写员:Paulina Kaniewska
审稿人:Rhonda Jacobs

当我怀上儿子时,
我被诊断出患有妊娠糖尿病

,这意味着
需要进行大量额外的检查和扫描,


每天检查四次血糖。

但有趣的是,

每次我得到一个新的测试结果
或数据点,如果你愿意,

它似乎
表明我不是糖尿病患者。

但是当我
在医院向医生提出这个问题时,

他坚持认为我是。

几个月过去了,我要求
进行另一次诊断测试

,结果为阴性。

于是我去了医院
,要求和医生谈谈。

我坐在医生的办公室里,
等他进来,

振作起来。

如果您曾经
看过现代谋杀之谜,

您就会知道法医科学家

竭尽全力防止他们的证据
受到污染。

你会看到他们穿着
西装、靴子和手套。

我们穿着这些东西是
为了阻止我们的物证

,比如 DNA 拭子、
指纹、鞋印、

接触与
案件无关的人或事物。

现在,我是一名法医科学家,

但今天我想谈谈
一种完全不同类型的污染

,那就是认知污染。


意味着我们的科学思维如何被我们所

了解的案例细节所污染。

当然,我们都喜欢
认为我们是纯粹客观的

,我们总是
做出 100% 科学的决定。

但有时
与科学无关的事情

会悄悄地影响我们的决策,

而我们甚至不
知道它已经发生了。

我的专业领域
是法医毒理学

,这意味着我
对毒品和酒精

感兴趣,嗯,
对毒品和酒精

以及它们
在死亡和犯罪中所起的作用非常感兴趣。

法医毒理学家可能处理的一种非常常见的案件类型

是在药物的影响下驾驶。

在这些情况下,通常情况下,我们会
从司机那里收到一份血样

并要求对其进行测试。

所以有一天,其中一个样本
到达我的实验室

,负责此案的警察
打电话给我说:

“我们已经抓到
这个 21 岁的男司机 9 次

了,因为他在毒品周围开车,

所以 我们很确定他是有罪的,

而在之前的九次中,
他服用了甲基苯丙胺

,你可能知道它是冰毒。

因此,在任何毒性案例
中,第一步都是决定进行哪种测试。

尽管你可能在 CSI 上看到过,

但没有一台神奇的“魔法机器

”可以测试任何一种药物
和毒药。

通常有一些
挑选和选择测试的元素

,我们希望选择
对案例最有用的测试。

那么,您认为哪种测试
在这种情况下最有用?

好吧,你们中的一些人
可能会坐在那里想:

“你的工作听起来很容易。
只需测试血液中的甲基苯丙胺。”

这是最明显的决定。

但这也是一个有偏见的决定,

因为它
受到了我被

告知司机之前的行为的影响。

我们称之为
“预期频率偏差”。

这就是陪审员没有被
告知以前的定罪的原因。

但即使我对司机

以前的行为一无所知

仅仅知道一些个人细节

也会导致同样
的偏见思维。

毒理学家有时会
根据

对吸毒人群的假设来选择测试。

基于年龄、性别、
种族甚至性取向的假设。

这些假设的问题
在于,它们只是这样,

它们只是猜测,

而且它们通常不是
基于任何特别科学的东西。

使用它们的危险

在于很
容易错过一些东西。

在这种情况下,我只能寻找甲基苯丙胺。

但如果这个结果是否定的呢?

如果司机拿了
别的东西,我想念它,

因为我看起来不正常,

因为我被
警察告诉我的事情所左右。

所以我需要更加开放
,我决定做一个筛查测试

,是的,可以看到甲基苯丙胺,

但也可以看到其他药物,
比如大麻和可卡因。

几天后,
我拿回了我的检测结果,

结果是甲基苯丙胺呈阳性。

“惊喜,惊喜,”
你可能会想,

但请记住我说过
这是一项筛查测试。

这意味着我所拥有的
只是一个简单的“是”或“否”

,结果是初步的。

这非常像
COVID-19 的横向流动测试。

阴性是可以的,但是阳性
需要用另一种方法来确认。

在法医毒理学中,
我们称这种方法为“质谱法”。

就像您对 COVID-19 的 PCR 测试一样,

它需要更长的时间
并且更昂贵。

它也给了我们
一个更复杂的答案。

因此,我们得到的不是简单的“是”或“否”,

而是
一张照片,一张电脑屏幕

上的照片,作为毒理学家,我的工作
就是将那张照片与另一张照片进行比较,

并进行某种“发现差异”。 ”

所以让我们看看我们的第一张照片
,我们称之为“质谱”。

现在,这是
一张甲基苯丙胺粉末的照片,

我知道的东西
肯定是甲基苯丙胺。

你可以在图片中看到
我们有三条垂直线

,每条线
代表一个数字。

左边是91,中间是119,
右边是150。

这三个数字加起来
告诉我这是甲基苯丙胺。

还有一个重要的细节

,那就是每条线
之间的比较高度。

你可以看到,最高的
实际上是中间的

119。下一个
最高的是右边的 150。

最短的是 91。

现在,让我们看看我们的案例。

希望您能立即
看到这里有一些额外的线条

不在甲基苯丙胺中。

所以我们有一个非常大的 88 岁,
另一个 60 岁,等等。

这些线条是什么意思?
它们重要吗?

它们只是一些
我可以忽略的垃圾吗?

如果你目光敏锐,
你也会看到

线条之间的比例不正确,

但右边的 150
比应该的短。

但是,我确实有我的三个号码。

这就是
真实案例样本的问题。

它们很混乱,很难解释。

这就是我们
用技术术语所说的“不确定的比赛”。

当结果不确定

时,案件信息
就会开始蔓延。

我认为这个样本来自

一名 21 岁的男性司机
,他已经被抓获

使用了 9 次甲基苯丙胺。

并且知道
我可能会下意识地将其

从“不确定”升级为“绝对积极”。

“是的,好的,那里有一些
额外的行,

但它们可能
只是一些我可以忽略的垃圾,

而且我拥有所有三个数字。

我会说这是积极的。”

现在,如果我认为这是
从疗养院的一位 92 岁的祖母那里拿走的怎么办?

我可能会下意识地将其
从“不确定”降级为“绝对负面”。

“一开始多出来的台词呢?
我真的不认为我可以忽略这些。

并且线条之间的比例
肯定是关闭的。

我会说这是负面的。”

如果我做了这两件事中的任何一件,
就会被称为“语境偏见”。

事实是,当完全相同的数据
被解释为

对 21 岁男性为阳性
但对 92 岁女性为阴性时,

这对刑事司法系统来说是一个真正的问题

同样重要的是要

知道并非
我获得的所有信息都是正确的。

人们并不总是
在犯罪后告诉警察真相。

我被告知的一些东西
基本上只是道听途说

,绝不会在法庭上被允许,

但它却可以影响
我的科学决策。

所以我打电话给警察
,我把消息告诉了他。

“我不能确定

这个样本中是否含有甲基苯丙胺。”

现在,这并不意味着它不存在。
这并不意味着司机没有这样做。

这意味着没有
足够的科学证据

表明样品中含有甲基苯丙胺。

在你看来,这对警方或检方来说可能不是
最好的结果

但这是基于科学的结果,

而不是基于
我被告知的司机的结果。

在我演讲的标题中,
我提到了一场战斗,

现在法医学界正在围绕

这些问题进行许多战斗。

现在法医病理学中有一个特别激烈的事件。

病理学家
与毒理学家密切合作,

因为他们的

工作是找出原因
,有时甚至是死亡方式。

但在我所在的领域,
同事会对我说:

“我们使用机器来获取数据。
机器是客观的,对吧?

那么我们的结果怎么会有偏差呢?”

但正如你今天看到的,问题
存在于键盘和椅子之间。

是的,机器给我们提供了客观的数据,

但决定这些数据意味着什么的是我们人类带着我们所有的
偏见。

我的同事也会对我说:

“我会忘记别人告诉我的。

当我做出科学决定时,我不会使用它
。”

可悲的是,我们只是
无法控制自己的潜意识。

所以让我们回到

我正坐着等待的那个医生办公室,那里
怀孕了。

一方面,

我对糖尿病有一个阳性
结果;

另一方面,我有
大量的负面数据。

然后
我没想到的事情发生了

,一位新医生走进了房间。

她看了看数据,
然后看着我说:

“这是误诊。
你没有妊娠糖尿病。”

我松了一口气,
但它让我思考。

“这里发生了什么?

为什么这两位医学专家会
查看相同的数据

并得出相反的结论?”

或者,换一种说法:

“是什么阻止了第一位医生
看到大量负面数据?”

阻止他的
是,他知道一些关于我的事情

,而第二位医生并不知道。

他知道我的家人患有糖尿病。

因为他知道这一点,所以
他无法忘记它。

他坚信,因为
我家里的其他人患有糖尿病,

所以我也一定患有糖尿病。

这被称为“确认偏差”

,它
是认知偏差难题的最后一块。

因为一旦我们了解了
有关患者或病例的某些信息,

我们就无法忘记它

,我们开始寻找

与我们所了解的事物相一致的数据或证据

我们开始忽略

与我们所学内容不一致的证据。

所以我
现在想问你一个问题。

鉴于我们

对认知污染
如何影响法医毒理学的了解,

现在不是我们开始
保护我们的证据免受其侵害的时候了吗?

谢谢你。

(掌声)