Steve Silberman The forgotten history of autism

Just after Christmas last year,

132 kids in California got the measles

by either visiting Disneyland

or being exposed to someone
who’d been there.

The virus then hopped the Canadian border,

infecting more than
100 children in Quebec.

One of the tragic things
about this outbreak

is that measles, which can be fatal
to a child with a weakened immune system,

is one of the most easily
preventable diseases in the world.

An effective vaccine against it

has been available for more
than half a century,

but many of the kids involved
in the Disneyland outbreak

had not been vaccinated

because their parents were afraid

of something allegedly even worse:

autism.

But wait – wasn’t the paper
that sparked the controversy

about autism and vaccines

debunked, retracted,

and branded a deliberate fraud

by the British Medical Journal?

Don’t most science-savvy people

know that the theory
that vaccines cause autism is B.S.?

I think most of you do,

but millions of parents worldwide

continue to fear that vaccines
put their kids at risk for autism.

Why?

Here’s why.

This is a graph of autism
prevalence estimates rising over time.

For most of the 20th century,

autism was considered
an incredibly rare condition.

The few psychologists and pediatricians
who’d even heard of it

figured they would get through
their entire careers

without seeing a single case.

For decades, the prevalence estimates
remained stable

at just three or four children in 10,000.

But then, in the 1990s,

the numbers started to skyrocket.

Fundraising organizations
like Autism Speaks

routinely refer to autism as an epidemic,

as if you could catch it
from another kid at Disneyland.

So what’s going on?

If it isn’t vaccines, what is it?

If you ask the folks down at
the Centers for Disease Control in Atlanta

what’s going on,

they tend to rely on phrases like
“broadened diagnostic criteria”

and “better case finding”

to explain these rising numbers.

But that kind of language

doesn’t do much to allay
the fears of a young mother

who is searching her
two-year-old’s face for eye contact.

If the diagnostic criteria
had to be broadened,

why were they so narrow
in the first place?

Why were cases of autism
so hard to find

before the 1990s?

Five years ago, I decided to try
to uncover the answers to these questions.

I learned that what happened

has less to do with the slow and cautious
progress of science

than it does with the seductive
power of storytelling.

For most of the 20th century,

clinicians told one story

about what autism is
and how it was discovered,

but that story turned out to be wrong,

and the consequences of it

are having a devastating impact
on global public health.

There was a second,
more accurate story of autism

which had been lost and forgotten

in obscure corners
of the clinical literature.

This second story tells us everything
about how we got here

and where we need to go next.

The first story starts with a child
psychiatrist at Johns Hopkins Hospital

named Leo Kanner.

In 1943, Kanner published a paper

describing 11 young patients
who seemed to inhabit private worlds,

ignoring the people around them,

even their own parents.

They could amuse themselves for hours

by flapping their hands
in front of their faces,

but they were panicked by little things

like their favorite toy
being moved from its usual place

without their knowledge.

Based on the patients
who were brought to his clinic,

Kanner speculated
that autism is very rare.

By the 1950s, as the world’s
leading authority on the subject,

he declared that he had seen
less than 150 true cases of his syndrome

while fielding referrals from
as far away as South Africa.

That’s actually not surprising,

because Kanner’s criteria
for diagnosing autism

were incredibly selective.

For example, he discouraged giving
the diagnosis to children who had seizures

but now we know that epilepsy
is very common in autism.

He once bragged that he had turned
nine out of 10 kids

referred to his office as autistic
by other clinicians

without giving them an autism diagnosis.

Kanner was a smart guy,

but a number of his theories
didn’t pan out.

He classified autism as a form
of infantile psychosis

caused by cold and unaffectionate parents.

These children, he said,

had been kept neatly
in a refrigerator that didn’t defrost.

At the same time, however,

Kanner noticed that some
of his young patients

had special abilities
that clustered in certain areas

like music, math and memory.

One boy in his clinic

could distinguish between 18 symphonies
before he turned two.

When his mother put on
one of his favorite records,

he would correctly declare,
“Beethoven!”

But Kanner took a dim view
of these abilities,

claiming that the kids
were just regurgitating things

they’d heard their pompous parents say,

desperate to earn their approval.

As a result, autism became
a source of shame and stigma for families,

and two generations of autistic children

were shipped off to institutions
for their own good,

becoming invisible to the world at large.

Amazingly, it wasn’t until the 1970s

that researchers began to test
Kanner’s theory that autism was rare.

Lorna Wing was a cognitive
psychologist in London

who thought that Kanner’s theory
of refrigerator parenting

were “bloody stupid,” as she told me.

She and her husband John were warm
and affectionate people,

and they had a profoundly
autistic daughter named Susie.

Lorna and John knew how hard it was
to raise a child like Susie

without support services,

special education,

and the other resources that are
out of reach without a diagnosis.

To make the case
to the National Health Service

that more resources were needed
for autistic children and their families,

Lorna and her colleague Judith Gould

decided to do something that should
have been done 30 years earlier.

They undertook a study of autism
prevalence in the general population.

They pounded the pavement
in a London suburb called Camberwell

to try to find autistic children
in the community.

What they saw made clear
that Kanner’s model was way too narrow,

while the reality of autism
was much more colorful and diverse.

Some kids couldn’t talk at all,

while others waxed on at length
about their fascination with astrophysics,

dinosaurs or the genealogy of royalty.

In other words, these children
didn’t fit into nice, neat boxes,

as Judith put it,

and they saw lots of them,

way more than Kanner’s monolithic model
would have predicted.

At first, they were at a loss
to make sense of their data.

How had no one noticed
these children before?

But then Lorna came upon a reference
to a paper that had been published

in German in 1944,

the year after Kanner’s paper,

and then forgotten,

buried with the ashes of a terrible time

that no one wanted to remember
or think about.

Kanner knew about this competing paper,

but scrupulously avoided
mentioning it in his own work.

It had never even
been translated into English,

but luckily, Lorna’s husband spoke German,

and he translated it for her.

The paper offered
an alternate story of autism.

Its author was a man named Hans Asperger,

who ran a combination clinic
and residential school

in Vienna in the 1930s.

Asperger’s ideas about teaching children
with learning differences

were progressive even
by contemporary standards.

Mornings at his clinic began
with exercise classes set to music,

and the children put on plays
on Sunday afternoons.

Instead of blaming parents
for causing autism,

Asperger framed it as a lifelong,
polygenetic disability

that requires compassionate forms
of support and accommodations

over the course of one’s whole life.

Rather than treating the kids
in his clinic like patients,

Asperger called them
his little professors,

and enlisted their help in developing
methods of education

that were particularly suited to them.

Crucially, Asperger viewed autism
as a diverse continuum

that spans an astonishing range
of giftedness and disability.

He believed that autism
and autistic traits are common

and always have been,

seeing aspects of this continuum
in familiar archetypes from pop culture

like the socially awkward scientist

and the absent-minded professor.

He went so far as to say,

it seems that for success
in science and art,

a dash of autism is essential.

Lorna and Judith realized that Kanner
had been as wrong about autism being rare

as he had been about parents causing it.

Over the next several years,

they quietly worked with
the American Psychiatric Association

to broaden the criteria for diagnosis

to reflect the diversity of what
they called “the autism spectrum.”

In the late ’80s and early 1990s,

their changes went into effect,

swapping out Kanner’s narrow model

for Asperger’s broad and inclusive one.

These changes weren’t
happening in a vacuum.

By coincidence, as Lorna and Judith
worked behind the scenes

to reform the criteria,

people all over the world were seeing
an autistic adult for the first time.

Before “Rain Man” came out in 1988,

only a tiny, ingrown circle of experts
knew what autism looked like,

but after Dustin Hoffman’s unforgettable
performance as Raymond Babbitt

earned “Rain Man” four Academy Awards,

pediatricians, psychologists,

teachers and parents all over the world
knew what autism looked like.

Coincidentally, at the same time,

the first easy-to-use clinical tests
for diagnosing autism were introduced.

You no longer had to have a connection
to that tiny circle of experts

to get your child evaluated.

The combination of “Rain Man,”

the changes to the criteria,
and the introduction of these tests

created a network effect,

a perfect storm of autism awareness.

The number of diagnoses started to soar,

just as Lorna and Judith predicted,
indeed hoped, that it would,

enabling autistic people
and their families

to finally get the support
and services they deserved.

Then Andrew Wakefield came along

to blame the spike
in diagnoses on vaccines,

a simple, powerful,

and seductively believable story

that was as wrong as Kanner’s theory

that autism was rare.

If the CDC’s current estimate,

that one in 68 kids in America
are on the spectrum, is correct,

autistics are one of the largest
minority groups in the world.

In recent years, autistic people
have come together on the Internet

to reject the notion that they
are puzzles to be solved

by the next medical breakthrough,

coining the term “neurodiversity”

to celebrate the varieties
of human cognition.

One way to understand neurodiversity

is to think in terms
of human operating systems.

Just because a P.C. is not running Windows
doesn’t mean that it’s broken.

By autistic standards,
the normal human brain

is easily distractable,

obsessively social,

and suffers from a deficit
of attention to detail.

To be sure, autistic people
have a hard time

living in a world not built for them.

[Seventy] years later, we’re still
catching up to Asperger,

who believed that the “cure”
for the most disabling aspects of autism

is to be found in understanding teachers,

accommodating employers,

supportive communities,

and parents who have faith
in their children’s potential.

An autistic [man]
named Zosia Zaks once said,

“We need all hands on deck
to right the ship of humanity.”

As we sail into an uncertain future,

we need every form
of human intelligence on the planet

working together to tackle
the challenges that we face as a society.

We can’t afford to waste a brain.

Thank you.

(Applause)

去年圣诞节刚过

,加利福尼亚州的 132 名儿童

通过参观迪士尼乐园

或接触到
去过那里的人感染了麻疹。

该病毒随后越过加拿大边境,

感染
了魁北克的 100 多名儿童。

这次爆发的悲剧之一

是麻疹对免疫系统较弱的儿童来说可能是致命的,它

是世界上最容易
预防的疾病之一。 半个多世纪以来,

一种有效的疫苗

已经问世

但许多
参与迪斯尼乐园爆发的

孩子没有接种疫苗,

因为他们的父母害怕

据称更糟糕的事情:

自闭症。

但是等等——
引发

关于自闭症和疫苗争议的论文不是被《英国医学杂志》

揭穿、撤回

并贴上蓄意欺诈

的标签吗?

难道大多数精通科学的人不

知道疫苗导致自闭症的理论是胡说八道吗?

我想你们中的大多数人都会这样做,

但是全世界数百万的父母

仍然担心疫苗
会使他们的孩子面临自闭症的风险。

为什么?

这就是为什么。

这是自闭症
患病率估计随时间上升的图表。

在 20 世纪的大部分时间里,

自闭症被认为
是一种极其罕见的疾病。

少数
甚至听说过这件事的心理学家和儿科医生

认为,他们会在

没有看到一个病例的情况下度过他们的整个职业生涯。

几十年来,患病率估计
保持稳定

,每 10,000 名儿童中只有 3 或 4 个。

但随后,在 1990 年代,

这一数字开始飙升。

像 Autism Speaks 这样的筹款组织

经常将自闭症称为一种流行病,

就好像你可以
从迪斯尼乐园的另一个孩子那里感染它一样。

发生什么了?

如果不是疫苗,那是什么?

如果你问
亚特兰大疾病控制中心

的工作人员发生了什么事,

他们往往会依靠
“扩大诊断标准”

和“更好的病例发现”等短语

来解释这些不断上升的数字。

但这种语言

并不能
减轻一位年轻母亲的恐惧,

她正在寻找她
两岁孩子的脸以寻找眼神交流。

如果
必须扩大诊断标准,

为什么一开始就如此
狭窄?

为什么

在 1990 年代之前很难找到自闭症病例?

五年前,我决定
尝试找出这些问题的答案。

我了解到,所

发生的事情与科学的缓慢而谨慎的进步关系不大,而与

讲故事的诱惑力有关。

在 20 世纪的大部分时间里,

临床医生讲述了一个

关于自闭症是什么
以及它是如何被发现的

故事,但事实证明这个故事是错误的

,其

后果对全球公共卫生产生了毁灭性的影响

还有第二个
更准确的自闭症故事,

在临床文献的晦涩角落中被遗忘和遗忘。

第二个故事告诉我们
关于我们如何到达这里

以及下一步需要去哪里的一切。

第一个故事开始
于约翰霍普金斯医院的一位

名叫 Leo Kanner 的儿童精神科医生。

1943 年,坎纳发表了一篇论文,

描述了 11 名年轻患者
,他们似乎生活在私人世界中,

无视周围的人,

甚至是他们自己的父母。

他们可以通过在脸前拍打手来娱乐自己几个小时

但他们会因为一些小事情而感到恐慌,

比如他们最喜欢的玩具
在他们不知情的情况下被从原来的位置移走

根据
被带到他诊所的病人,

坎纳
推测自闭症是非常罕见的。

到 1950 年代,作为该主题的世界
领先权威,

他宣称他在接受远至南非
的转诊时看到的真实病例不到 150 例

这实际上并不奇怪,

因为 Kanner
诊断自闭症

的标准非常具有选择性。

例如,他不
鼓励对癫痫发作的儿童进行诊断,

但现在我们知道癫痫
在自闭症中很常见。

他曾经吹嘘说,他已经变成
了十分之九的孩子,其他临床医生

将他的办公室称为自闭症

而没有给他们进行自闭症诊断。

坎纳是个聪明人,

但他的一些理论
并没有成功。

他将自闭症归类为一种

由冷漠和冷漠的父母引起的婴儿精神病。

他说,这些

孩子被整齐
地放在不解冻的冰箱里。

然而,与此同时,

坎纳注意到
他的一些年轻患者

具有集中在

音乐、数学和记忆等特定领域的特殊能力。

他诊所里的一个男孩在两岁之前

就能分辨出 18 首交响乐

当他的母亲
放上他最喜欢的一张唱片时,

他会正确地宣布:
“贝多芬!”

但坎纳对这些能力持悲观态度

声称孩子
们只是在重复

他们从自负的父母所说的话,

急切地想要获得他们的认可。

结果,自闭症成为
家庭耻辱和耻辱的根源

,两代自闭症儿童

为了自己的利益被送往机构,

成为整个世界的隐形人。

令人惊讶的是,直到 1970 年代

,研究人员才开始检验
Kanner 的自闭症罕见理论。

Lorna Wing
是伦敦的一位认知心理学家,

她认为 Kanner
的冰箱

育儿理论“非常愚蠢”,正如她告诉我的那样。

她和她的丈夫约翰是热情
而深情的人

,他们有一个
患有严重自闭症的女儿,名叫苏西。

Lorna 和 John 知道,如果

没有支持服务、

特殊教育

和其他
未经诊断就无法获得的资源,要抚养像 Susie 这样的孩子是多么困难。

为了
向国家卫生服务局

证明
自闭症儿童及其家人需要更多资源,

洛娜和她的同事朱迪思·古尔德

决定做一些本应
在 30 年前做的事情。

他们对普通人群的自闭症患病率进行了研究

他们
在伦敦郊区坎伯韦尔的人行道上敲打

,试图在社区中寻找自闭症儿童

他们所看到的清楚地
表明,坎纳的模型过于狭窄,

而自闭症的现实
则更加丰富多彩和多样化。

有些孩子根本不会说话,而另一些孩子则滔滔不绝地

谈论他们对天体物理学、

恐龙或皇室家谱的迷恋。

换句话说,这些孩子
不适合朱迪思所说的漂亮、整洁的盒子,

而且他们看到了很多

,远远超过了坎纳的整体模型
所预测的。

起初,他们
无法理解他们的数据。

以前怎么没人注意到
这些孩子?

但后来 Lorna 提到
了一篇论文,该论文

于 1944 年以德语发表,

即 Kanner 论文的第二年,

然后被遗忘,

埋葬在一个没有人想记住或想念的可怕时代的灰烬中

Kanner 知道这篇竞争论文,


在他自己的工作中小心翼翼地避免提及它。

它甚至
从未被翻译成英文,

但幸运的是,洛娜的丈夫会说德语

,他为她翻译了它。

这篇论文提供
了另一个关于自闭症的故事。

它的作者是一位名叫汉斯·阿斯伯格 (Hans Asperger) 的人,

他于 1930 年代在维也纳经营着一家综合诊所
和寄宿学校

。 即使按照当代标准,

阿斯伯格关于教
有学习差异的孩子的想法

也是进步的

在他的诊所里,早上
的运动课以音乐开始

,孩子们
在周日下午上演戏剧。 阿斯伯格没有将自闭症

归咎于父母,而是将

其描述为一种终生的
多基因残疾

,需要在一个人的一生
中给予富有同情心的支持和适应

。 阿斯伯格没有

像对待病人一样对待诊所里的孩子,而是

称他们为
小教授,

并在开发

特别适合他们的教育方法方面寻求他们的帮助。

至关重要的是,阿斯伯格认为自闭症
是一个多样化的连续体

,跨越了惊人
的天赋和残疾范围。

他认为自闭症
和自闭症特征很常见,

而且一直都是,从流行文化中熟悉的原型中

看到这种连续体的各个方面,

比如社交尴尬的科学家

和心不在焉的教授。

他甚至说,

似乎要
在科学和艺术上取得成功

,一点自闭症是必不可少的。

Lorna 和 Judith 意识到,Kanner
认为自闭症罕见,

就像他认为父母导致自闭症一样是错误的。

在接下来的几年里,

他们悄悄地
与美国精神病学协会

合作,扩大诊断标准,

以反映
他们所谓的“自闭症谱系”的多样性。

在 80 年代末和 1990 年代初,

他们的变化开始生效,

将 Kanner 的狭隘模型替换

为 Asperger 广泛而包容的模型。

这些变化不是
凭空发生的。

巧合的是,当 Lorna 和 Judith
在幕后工作

以改革标准时,

全世界的人们
第一次看到了自闭症成年人。

在 1988 年《雨人》问世之前,

只有一小群内向的专家
知道自闭症是什么样子,

但在达斯汀·霍夫曼
饰演雷蒙德·巴比特的令人难忘的表演为

《雨人》赢得了四项奥斯卡金像奖之后,

儿科医生、心理学家、

老师和家长都 全世界
都知道自闭症是什么样子的。

巧合的是,与此同时,引入

了第一个易于使用的
用于诊断自闭症的临床测试。

您不再需要
与那一小群专家建立联系

来评估您的孩子。

结合“雨人”

的标准变化,
以及这些测试的引入,

创造了一种网络效应,

一场自闭症意识的完美风暴。

正如 Lorna 和 Judith 预测的那样,诊断的数量开始飙升,并且
确实希望,这将使

自闭症患者
及其

家人最终获得他们应得的支持
和服务。

然后,安德鲁·韦克菲尔德(Andrew Wakefield)


诊断的激增归咎于疫苗,这

是一个简单、有力

且令人信服的故事

,与坎纳

关于自闭症罕见的理论一样错误。

如果疾病预防控制中心目前的估计,

即美国每 68 个孩子中就有一个
是正确的,那么

自闭症患者是世界上最大的
少数群体之一。

近年来,自闭症患者
在互联网上聚集在一起

,反对他们

下一个医学突破需要解决的难题的观念,

创造了“神经多样性”一词


庆祝人类认知的多样性。

理解神经多样性的一种方法

是从
人类操作系统的角度进行思考。

仅仅因为一台PC 未运行 Windows
并不意味着它已损坏。

按照自闭症的标准
,正常人的大脑

很容易分心,

痴迷于社交,

并且缺乏
对细节的关注。

可以肯定的是,自闭症
患者很难

生活在一个不是为他们而建的世界中。

[70] 年后,我们仍在
追赶阿斯伯格,

他认为,
自闭症最致残方面的“治愈”方法

在于理解教师、

包容雇主、

支持社区

和相信他们的父母
。 孩子的潜力。

一位
名叫 Zosia Zaks 的自闭症患者曾经说过:

“我们需要所有人齐心协力,
才能让人类之船恢复正常。”

当我们驶入一个不确定的未来时,

我们需要
地球上各种形式的人类智慧

共同努力,以应对
我们作为一个社会所面临的挑战。

我们不能浪费大脑。

谢谢你。

(掌声)