How does the Rorschach inkblot test work Damion Searls

Take a look at this image.

What might this be?

A frightening monster?

Two friendly bears?

Or something else entirely?

For nearly a century,

ten inkblots like these have been used

as what seems like an almost
mystical personality test.

Long kept confidential for psychologists
and their patients,

the mysterious images were said to draw
out the workings of a person’s mind.

But what can inkblots really tell us,

and how does this test work?

Invented in the early 20th century
by Swiss psychiatrist Hermann Rorschach,

the Rorschach Test is actually less about
the specific things we see,

and more about our general approach
to perception.

As an amateur artist

Hermann was fascinated by how visual
perception varies from person to person.

He carried this interest to
medical school,

where he learned all our senses
are deeply connected.

He studied how our process of perception
doesn’t just register sensory inputs,

but transforms them.

And when he started working at a
mental hospital in eastern Switzerland,

he began designing a series
of puzzling images

to gain new insight into this
enigmatic process.

Using his inkblot paintings,

Rorschach began quizzing hundreds
of healthy subjects

and psychiatric patients with
the same question:

what might this be?

However, it wasn’t what the test subjects
saw that was most important to Rorschach,

but rather, how they approached the task.

Which parts of the image did they
focus on or ignore?

Did they see the image moving?

Did the color on some inkblots help them
give better answers,

or distract and overwhelm them?

He developed a system to code
people’s responses,

reducing the wide range of interpretations
to a few manageable numbers.

Now he had empirical measures to quantify
all kinds of test takers:

the creative and imaginative,

the detail-oriented, the
big-picture perceivers,

and flexible participants able
to adapt their approach.

Some people would get stuck,

offering the same answer
for multiple blots.

Others gave unusual and
delightful descriptions.

Responses were as varied as the inkblots,

which offered different kinds of
perceptual problems–

some easier to interpret than others.

But analyzing the test-taker’s
overall approach

yielded real insights into
their psychology.

And as Rorschach tested more
and more people,

patterns began to pile up.

Healthy subjects with the same
personalities

often took remarkably similar approaches.

Patients suffering from the same
mental illnesses

also performed similarly,

making the test a reliable
diagnostic tool.

It could even diagnose some conditions

difficult to pinpoint with other
available methods.

In 1921,

Rorschach published his coding system
alongside the ten blots he felt

gave the most nuanced picture of people’s
perceptual approach.

Over the next several decades,

the test became wildly popular in
countries around the world.

By the 1960s,

it had been officially administered
millions of times in the U.S. alone.

Unfortunately, less than a year after
publishing the test,

Hermann Rorschach had died suddenly.

Without its inventor to keep it on track,

the test he had methodically gathered
so much data to support

began to be used in all sorts
of speculative ways.

Researchers gave the test
to Nazi war criminals,

hoping to unlock the psychological roots
of mass murder.

Anthropologists showed the images to
remote communities

as a sort of universal personality test.

Employers made prejudiced hiring decisions
based on reductive decoding charts.

As the test left clinics and entered
popular culture

its reputation among medical
professionals plummeted,

and the blots began to fall
out of clinical use.

Today, the test is still controversial,

and many people assume
it has been disproven.

But a massive 2013 review of all the
existing Rorschach research

showed that when administered properly
the test yields valid results,

which can help diagnose mental illness

or round out a patient’s
psychological profile.

It’s hardly a stand-alone key
to the human mind–

no test is.

But its visual approach and lack
of any single right answer

continue to help psychologists paint
a more nuanced picture

of how people see the world.

Bringing us one step closer

to understanding the patterns
behind our perceptions.

看看这张图片。

这可能是什么?

可怕的怪物?

两只友好的熊?

还是完全不同的东西?

近一个世纪以来,

十个像这样的墨迹被

用作几乎
神秘的性格测试。

长期以来

这些神秘的图像一直为心理学家及其患者保密,据说这些图像可以
勾勒出一个人的思维运作方式。

但是墨迹真正能告诉我们

什么,这个测试是如何工作的?

由瑞士精神病学家赫尔曼·罗夏 (Hermann Rorschach) 于 20 世纪初发明

的罗夏测验实际上不是关于
我们所看到的具体事物,

而是更多关于我们感知的一般
方法。

作为一名业余艺术家,

赫尔曼着迷于视觉
感知因人而异。

他把这种兴趣带到了
医学院,

在那里他了解到我们所有的感官
都紧密相连。

他研究了我们的感知过程
如何不仅记录感官输入,

而且改变它们。

当他开始
在瑞士东部的一家精神病院工作时,

他开始设计一系列
令人费解的图像,

以对这个神秘的过程有了新的认识

使用他的墨迹画,

罗夏开始向数百
名健康受试者

和精神病患者
询问同一个问题:

这可能是什么?

然而,
对罗夏来说,最重要的并不是测试对象看到了什么,

而是他们如何完成任务。

他们关注或忽略了图像的哪些部分

他们看到图像移动了吗?

一些墨迹上的颜色是否有助于他们
给出更好的答案,

或者分散他们的注意力并让他们不知所措?

他开发了一个系统来编码
人们的反应,

将广泛的解释减少
到几个可管理的数字。

现在,他有了量化
各种应试者的实证措施

:创造性和想象力

、注重细节、
大局观

以及能够
适应他们方法的灵活参与者。

有些人会卡住,为多个印迹

提供相同的答案

其他人则给出了不同寻常且
令人愉快的描述。

反应就像墨迹一样多种多样,

它提供了不同类型的
感知问题——

有些问题比其他问题更容易解释。

但分析应试者的
整体方法可以让我们

真正了解
他们的心理。

随着罗夏测试
越来越多的人,

模式开始堆积起来。

具有相同性格的健康受试者

通常采取非常相似的方法。

患有相同
精神疾病的患者

也表现类似,

使该测试成为可靠的
诊断工具。

它甚至可以诊断出一些

其他可用方法难以确定的
情况。

1921 年,

罗夏公布了他的编码系统
以及他认为

最细致入微的人们
感知方法的十个印迹。

在接下来的几十年里,

该测试
在世界各国广受欢迎。

到 1960 年代,仅在美国,

它就被正式管理了
数百万次。

不幸的是,在发布测试后不到一年

赫尔曼·罗夏就突然去世了。

没有它的发明者来跟踪它,

他有条不紊地收集了
这么多数据来支持的测试

开始以
各种推测的方式使用。

研究人员
对纳粹战犯进行了测试,

希望能够解开
大屠杀的心理根源。

人类学家将这些图像展示给
偏远社区,

作为一种普遍的性格测试。

雇主根据还原解码图表做出有偏见的招聘决定

随着测试离开诊所并进入
流行文化,

它在医学
专业人士中的声誉直线

下降,印迹开始
不再用于临床。

今天,该测试仍然存在争议

,许多人认为
它已被证明是错误的。

但 2013 年对所有现有罗夏研究的大规模审查

表明,如果管理
得当,测试会产生有效的结果,

这有助于诊断精神疾病

或完善患者的
心理状况。

它几乎不是
人类思维的独立钥匙——

没有测试。

但它的视觉方法和
缺乏任何单一的正确答案

继续帮助心理学家描绘
出人们如何看待世界的更细致入微的

画面。

让我们更

接近于理解
我们感知背后的模式。