Why do people fear the wrong things Gerd Gigerenzer

A new drug reduces
the risk of heart attacks by 40%.

Shark attacks are up by a factor of two.

Drinking a liter of soda per day
doubles your chance of developing cancer.

These are all examples of relative risk,

a common way risk
is presented in news articles.

Risk evaluation is a complicated tangle
of statistical thinking

and personal preference.

One common stumbling block
is the difference between

relative risks like these
and what are called absolute risks.

Risk is the likelihood
that an event will occur.

It can be expressed
as either a percentage—

for example, that heart attacks
occur in 11% of men

between the ages of 60 and 79—

or as a rate— that one in two million
divers along Australia’s western coast

will suffer a fatal shark bite each year.

These numbers express
the absolute risk of heart attacks

and shark attacks in these groups.

Changes in risk can be expressed
in relative or absolute terms.

For example, a review in 2009
found that mammography screenings

reduced the number of breast cancer deaths
from five women in one thousand to four.

The absolute risk reduction
was about .1%.

But the relative risk reduction
from 5 cases of cancer mortality to four

is 20%.

Based on reports of this higher number,

people overestimated
the impact of screening.

To see why the difference between
the two ways of expressing risk matters,

let’s consider
the hypothetical example of a drug

that reduces heart attack risk by 40%.

Imagine that out of a group
of 1,000 people

who didn’t take the new drug,
10 would have heart attacks.

The absolute risk
is 10 out of 1,000, or 1%.

If a similar group of 1,000 people
did take the drug,

the number of heart attacks would be six.

In other words, the drug could prevent
four out of ten heart attacks—

a relative risk reduction of 40%.

Meanwhile, the absolute risk
only dropped from 1% to 0.6%—

but the 40% relative risk decrease
sounds a lot more significant.

Surely preventing
even a handful of heart attacks,

or any other negative outcome,
is worthwhile— isn’t it?

Not necessarily.

The problem is that choices
that reduce some risks

can put you in the path of others.

Suppose the heart-attack drug caused
cancer in one half of 1% of patients.

In our group of 1,000 people,

four heart attacks
would be prevented by taking the drug,

but there would be
five new cases of cancer.

The relative reduction
in heart attack risk sounds substantial

and the absolute risk of cancer
sounds small,

but they work out
to about the same number of cases.

In real life,

everyone’s individual evaluation of risk
will vary

depending on
their personal circumstances.

If you know you have a family history
of heart disease

you might be more strongly motivated
to take a medication

that would lower your heart-attack risk,

even knowing it provided
only a small reduction in absolute risk.

Sometimes, we have to decide between
exposing ourselves to risks

that aren’t directly comparable.

If, for example, the heart attack drug
carried a higher risk

of a debilitating,
but not life-threatening,

side effect like migraines
rather than cancer,

our evaluation of whether that risk
is worth taking might change.

And sometimes there isn’t necessarily
a correct choice:

some might say even a minuscule risk
of shark attack is worth avoiding,

because all you’d miss out on
is an ocean swim,

while others wouldn’t even consider
skipping a swim

to avoid an objectively tiny risk
of shark attack.

For all these reasons,
risk evaluation is tricky at baseline,

and reporting on risk can be misleading,

especially when it shares some numbers
in absolute terms

and others in relative terms.

Understanding how these measures work

will help you cut through
some of the confusion

and better evaluate risk.

一种新药可
将心脏病发作的风险降低 40%。

鲨鱼攻击增加了两倍。

每天喝一升苏打水
会使你患癌症的几率增加一倍。

这些都是相对风险的例子,这

是新闻文章中呈现风险的常见方式。

风险评估
是统计思维

和个人偏好的复杂纠结。

一个常见的绊脚石

此类相对风险
与所谓的绝对风险之间的差异。

风险
是事件发生的可能性。

它可以用百分比来表示——

例如,心脏病发作
发生在 11%

的 60 至 79 岁之间的男性中——

或者用比率表示——
澳大利亚西海岸的两百万潜水员中

就有一人将遭受致命的鲨鱼咬伤 每年。

这些数字表达

这些群体心脏病发作和鲨鱼袭击的绝对风险。

风险的变化可以
用相对或绝对的方式表示。

例如,2009 年的一项审查
发现,乳房 X 光检查

将乳腺癌死亡
人数从千分之五减少到四位。

绝对风险
降低约 0.1%。

但从 5 例癌症死亡率到 4 例的相对风险降低了

20%。

根据这个较高数字的报告,

人们高估
了筛查的影响。

要了解为什么
两种表达风险的方式之间的差异很重要,

让我们
考虑一个假设的例子,即一种药物

可以将心脏病发作风险降低 40%。

想象一下,
在 1000

名未服用新药的人中,有
10 人会心脏病发作。

绝对风险
是千分之十,或 1%。

如果一个类似的 1000 人
确实服用了这种药物,

心脏病发作的次数将是六次。

换句话说,这种药物可以预防
十分之四的心脏病发作——

相对风险降低了 40%。

与此同时,绝对风险
仅从 1% 下降到 0.6%,

但 40% 的相对风险下降
听起来更显着。

当然,
即使是预防少数心脏病发作

或任何其他负面结果,
也是值得的——不是吗?

不必要。

问题
在于,降低某些风险的选择

可能会让你走上其他人的道路。

假设心脏病药物导致
1% 的一半患者患上癌症。

在我们的 1,000 人小组中

,服用该药可以预防四次心脏病发作,

但会出现
五例新的癌症病例。

心脏病发作风险的相对降低听起来很可观

,癌症的绝对风险
听起来很小,

但它们
的病例数大致相同。

在现实生活中,

每个人对风险的个人评估
会因

个人情况而异。

如果您知道自己
有心脏病家族史,

那么您可能会更有动力

服用可以降低心脏病发作风险的药物,

即使您知道这种药物
只会略微降低绝对风险。

有时,我们必须在
将自己暴露于

无法直接比较的风险之间做出决定。

例如,如果心脏病发作
药物具有更高

的使人衰弱
但不会危及生命的

副作用(如偏头痛
而不是癌症)的风险,那么

我们对这种风险
是否值得承担的评估可能会改变。

有时不一定
是正确的选择:

有些人可能会说,即使是很小
的鲨鱼袭击风险也值得避免,

因为你错过的
只是一次海洋游泳,

而另一些人甚至不会考虑
跳过游泳

来避免 鲨鱼袭击的客观风险很小

由于所有这些原因,
风险评估在基线时很棘手,

并且风险报告可能会产生误导,

特别是当它以绝对值表示某些数字

而以相对值表示其他数字时。

了解这些措施的工作原理

将帮助您消除
一些困惑

并更好地评估风险。