Do we see the world as it really is

[Music]

okay uh

thank you thank you very much indeed um

so the theme of

the theme of today’s session uh is

distortion

and as i was thinking about this uh it

occurred to me that the very concept of

distortion

implies that there’s an original or a

real version of something

and a distorted version and

that sort of line of thinking about the

being an original or a real version

versus a distorted version really begs

or

poses or brings up one of the very

oldest questions in psychology and

philosophy

which is of course do we see the world

as it really is

so do our powers of perception and

reason

really give us a sound and accurate

picture of the world

or is it the case that our perceptions

and our

reasoning uh are always incomplete

and flawed so that perhaps we are only

ever able to gain an incomplete

or perhaps a picture a picture of a

shadow of reality

which is really what plato thought uh

approximately two and a half thousand

years ago

of course the everyday answer the

everyday answer to this question is that

perception certainly

seems to work perfectly accurately and

perfectly well

despite all the glare i can see a world

of people i can

open my eyes and see a world of people

and objects and be very very confident

that i can take

a few steps forward and walk around the

obstacles in front of me pick

up the objects and talk to the people

talk to the people that are here

so so perception seems to work

perfectly well we seem to open our eyes

it seems simple and effortless

but is it really that simple is it is it

really that straightforward

well an approach which is available to

us here in the 21st century which of

course wasn’t available

to plato uh and his contemporaries is to

look inside the skull and ask well how

much brain power

is involved in the apparently simple act

of just opening our eyes

and looking out at the world well if we

look at

if we do look inside the skull what we

can see is the

is the surface of the cerebral cor the

surface of the brain cerebral cortex

is divided into four main lobes as we

can see here the occipital parietal

frontal and temporal lobes and it

turns out that all four of those lobes

of the cerebral cortex are critically

important for vision not only that

of course underneath the cortex there’s

a whole range of subcortical brain

structures

and it turns out that several of those

are also

critically important for vision

so uh i’ve always been very surprised by

this it’s always been surprising to me

that so much of the brain

which we think of as our organ of

thinking uh

is really devoted to this apparently

simple task

of just seeing of of perceiving the

world

of course on the other hand it’s also

true that when we think

about thinking and when we talk about

thinking

we often do that in highly visual terms

we talk about ideas that are opaque or

hazy

or perhaps crystal clear we talk about

theories that may be insightful

or enlightening and we talk about people

who are very bright

or perhaps if we want to insult them we

might say that they’re a bit dim

so so perhaps you can see what i mean

here you can and you can see

where i’m going so so vision is

something that in our everyday lives we

take completely for granted

and we use phrases such as seeing is

believing

on the other hand perhaps

like the character neo in the classic

movie the matrix

we can occasionally become aware of

become aware of

little cracks and fissures in the

structure and sort of coherent

of our visual reality so if we look at

this picture here which is a still from

the matrix we can see that neo

uh is holding up four fingers and i hope

that all seems completely obvious to you

he’s holding up four fingers there and

if we move to this slide and i ask you

again

how many fingers am i showing you here

again the answer seems completely

obvious

well obviously there’s three but if we

look at a more complete picture

uh we can see what but well actually

perhaps the answer might be

two well you might say well

well that’s just a visual trick let’s

look at something a bit more

straightforward let’s

let’s just get this straight and if i

ask you are these lines

perfectly are these lines straight and

parallel

again the answer seems very obvious

obviously they’re

they’re straight and parallel um but

what about these ones if we look at

these ones do you think these ones are

straight and parallel

well as i look at them there’s no way

that they look straight and parallel

they seem to be sort of waving all over

the place

but if we superimpose the two slides

we can see that in fact both sets of

lines

are perfectly straight and perfectly

parallel

so this is a very well known illusion

it’s called the cafe wall illusion and

it’s called that

because this man here very eminent uh

british psychologist called richard

gregory

was having his lunch one day several

many decades ago in bristol

and he glanced across and saw this uh

this particular cafe wall actually

noticed the illusion described it and

then studied it

and a little bit a little bit closer to

home actually here so here’s a much more

scaled up version of the cafe wall

illusion

uh which i believe you can see on the

side of this building

in melbourne okay so uh

so perhaps seeing isn’t always believing

and one of the things that has a strong

influence in

on how we see things is the context in

which we see them

so if we look at these two orange discs

here on this slide

this is another very well known illusion

called the ebbinghaus illusion

we can see that the one on the right one

on the right looks relatively big the

one on the left looks relatively small

but of course they’ll be looking at the

one on the right in the context of these

somewhat small little gray little gray

discs whereas the one on the left is

we’re

looking at that in the context of these

much larger discs

and if we take that context away what we

can see

is that in fact the two discs are

exactly the same

size so it turns out that context

that context can have some very powerful

influences on the way

we see things we often talk about

people who see things in black and white

and the reason we say that

is that black and white do seem like

such polar opposites and you couldn’t

possibly argue

you know that black is white or that

white is black

but let’s just have a look at this

illustration here which is a very clever

demonstration developed

by a vision scientist called edward h

adelson

so what we can see here is a black and

white

checkerboard and if i ask you

which of these two squares is darker

square

a or square b so which one looks darker

well i hope you’ll agree

that a clearly looks quite a bit darker

but if we look at this illustration a

little

more carefully what we can see is that

we’re looking at

b inside the shadow that’s being cast by

this

large uh green cylinder

so let’s just see if a is really darker

than b

by taking away the visual context

of the rest of the figure including the

shadow so let’s leave the

lose the left hand side the right hand

side

some of the context around the two

squares and we take away the last little

bit

and we can see that in fact a and b are

exactly the same brightness

so we can argue actually that black is

white and white is black

and it’s not in and it’s not an entirely

silly thing to uh

to say and just to show you that there’s

no there’s no real jiggery poker here

keep looking at a and b and let’s put

the context back in and as we add in the

shadows and the rest of the figure

you can see that when we add the context

add the context back in again again

a looks totally different to b okay

so what we can see from this uh is that

context can have these very

powerful effects on the way we see

things visually

so what about how we see the world of

ideas and beliefs

can context also have powerful effects

in that more abstract

cognitive domain and i’ll illustrate uh

i’ll illustrate this by talking about a

very well known study

carried out by these two psychologists

here daniel kahneman and amos taversky

and as many of you will know uh kahneman

went on several years

several years after this study was many

years after this study was published to

uh

to win the nobel prize so back in the

early 1970s

kahneman and tversky dreamt up this

scenario

which as you can see is uh spookily

relevant

to the situation in which we find

ourselves today

what they asked their research

participants to do

was to imagine that the country they

were in new zealand in this case

is preparing for the outbreak of an

especially

virulent virus and people were told if

urgent steps are not taken

we’re going to lose 600 lives

so then the participants were asked were

presented with this

choice they were asked to choose whether

or not we should adopt

program a or program b so pro so the

programs could be to do with

alternative vaccines or it could be to

do with lockdown

protocols and so on uh some procedure

for reducing the impact of the virus

and what the people were told was well

if we go with program

a 200 people are going to be saved

whereas if we go with program b there’s

a one in three probability that 600

people will be saved and a two in three

probability

that no people will be saved so when

faced

with that choice a large majority

72 percent chose program a

they preferred the certainty of saving

those 200 lives however

what kahneman and tversky also did was

to recruit a second group of

participants

and and the second the second group was

given this

choice they were told if we adopt

program a

400 people are going to lose their lives

whereas if we adopt program b we have a

one in three probability that no one

will die

and a two in three probability that 600

people will die

when faced with these choices a

similarly large majority in fact a

slightly larger majority

went for program b but if you look at

these

alternatives carefully or even not that

carefully you can see that in fact the

information is exactly the same

losing 400 400 of the 600 lives is

exactly the same

as saving 200 of the 600 lives and the

information in pro

for program b is also in fact identical

so what we can see from this uh is that

is that if we present uh essentially

identical phrase

identical information in different

frameworks in different

contexts we can produce radical changes

in people’s preferences

and judgments so if we present

yeah we can produce these very strong

changes

just by changing whether or not we

present the information in terms of

saving lives or in terms of losing lives

so we’ve got an election coming up here

in a few weeks time and i gather there’s

another election

happening in america at the beginning of

november

and of course if any of the candidates

and parties in those

elections won a majority in the mid or

high

70 percents they’d be absolutely

delighted and ecstatic

so so what we can see from this

is is that these context effects can

produce very powerful and very dramatic

effect

very dramatic influences uh on people’s

preferences

so context can be critical in perception

even when you’re judging something that

seems simple and straightforward

such as how bright something is how

large something is

whether or not we have a straight line

but also

when we’re making these complex

high-level judgments

about what we should do when faced with

a global pandemic

so so what what can we learn from this

what can we conclude from this

you might be a little bit worried at

this point and think well

can we ever really trust our own

perceptions and our own judgments

is our view of the world really at the

mercy

of the particular and specific context

that we just happen to find ourselves in

you might worry and feel like w b yates

that you’re

that your head is spinning uh at this

point because

the things you thought you could see

clearly and the foundations of your

beliefs and judgments perhaps suddenly

they don’t seem so

firm anymore and perhaps they’re

suddenly more fragile

and and perhaps prone to disintegrating

well i think that would be that would be

a very negative con

a very negative uh conclusion but i and

i think there’s a much more

positive way of thinking about

the way in which context can influence

our perceptions and our judgments and

i’d like to illustrate that

by stepping away from psychology for a

few moments and stepping into the realm

of physics

and of course the the second part of our

question is really centrally about the

nature of the world and of course

questions about the nature of the world

are really really in the province of

physics

not the province of psychology so um

back in 2016 uh this man here carla

rivelli

wrote a fascinating book called reality

is not what it seems and one of the

points that ravelli

made in this book is that we can view

reality from multiple perspectives

so we have our everyday perspective

where we have a world of

solid objects

objects appear solid there and they

exist at a particular

place in a particular location in space

and at a particular point in time but

ravelli points out that that breaks down

completely

when we start to think about what’s

happening at the

almost unimaginably tiny uh

tiny spatial scale and brief time scale

of the way in which fundamental

particles interact with each other

and of course our perceptions of time

and space are also radically different

when we consider

the the much the almost unimaginably

large time scales where stars and whole

galaxies

may form so so reality so we can see

reality from multiple perspectives

so so i think the the the message that

i’d like to

finish with is to say that

when we approach a really big question

such as do we see the world as it really

is we’re very unlikely to get a complete

answer

by looking at it from a single

perspective if we want

if we really want to gain a more

complete uh

a better and more complete answer we’re

going to

we’re almost certainly going to need to

step out of our own

uh particular contextual silo as it were

and that silo

might be your at your own academic or

scientific discipline

of course we of course at the moment

we’re living

in in very difficult and challenging and

potentially very divisive

times so perhaps there’s also a more

perhaps there’s also a broader lesson to

to learn here about trying to think

about

issues from a different perspective and

of course that different perspective

might be the perspective of somebody

who’s at the opposite end of the

political spectrum to the

to the location where you are or it

might be somebody from a different

social or cultural

group psychologists refer to this as

perspective

taking the ability to see something

as it might be seen from a different

point of view

and recently the neuroscience of

perspective taking it has

made some quite interesting advances by

looking at the brain regions that become

active

when you imagine looking at a visual

object

from a different point of view or if you

imagine

adopting a different belief from the

belief that you currently

hold and interestingly uh some

very common areas are lighted up when

you do both of those things when you

imagine a different visual point of view

and you imagine a different cognitive

point of view

so that’s perspective taking it it

emerges quite early in childhood

in stages between the ages of about two

and

five years old um and the final thought

i’d like to leave you with

is that we know that young children can

do perspective

taking but perhaps it also might be

something that’s been

going to be very beneficial and helpful

for us to practice that a little bit

more

as adults as well as as well as when we

were children

and that’s and thank you very much for

listening

[音乐]

好的呃

谢谢谢谢 非常感谢 确实嗯

所以

今天的主题呃是

失真

,我在想这个的时候突然想到失真这个

概念

本身就意味着有原作或者原作

某物的真实版本

和扭曲版本,以及

关于原创或真实版本

与扭曲版本的那种思维方式确实乞求

提出或提出

心理学和哲学中最古老的问题之一,

这当然是 我们看到世界

的本来面目,

所以我们的感知和

推理能力

真的给了我们关于世界的完整和准确的

画面,

或者我们的感知

推理总是不完整

和有缺陷的,所以也许我们只是

永远能够获得一张不完整

的图片,或者一张图片,一张

现实阴影的图片,

这确实是柏拉图认为的,呃

,大约两千

年前

,当然,每一个

每天回答这个问题的日常答案是,尽管有所有的眩光,

感知

似乎确实非常准确和完美地工作

我可以看到一个

人的世界我可以

睁开眼睛看到一个人和物体的世界,

并且非常非常有

信心 我可以

向前走几步,绕过

我面前的障碍物,

捡起物体,和人们

交谈,和这里的人交谈,

所以感知似乎工作得

很好 毫不费力,

但它真的那么简单吗?它真的那么

简单吗?这是我们在 21 世纪可以使用的一种方法,

柏拉图呃和他的同时代人当然无法使用这种方法,那就是

看看头骨内部并问清楚如何

很多脑力

都参与到这个看似简单的动作

大脑皮层的 ACE 大脑皮层的

表面

分为四个主叶,我们

可以在这里看到枕顶叶

额叶和颞叶,

事实证明,大脑皮层的所有四个叶

对视力都至关重要,而不是

只有在皮层下面当然有

一系列的皮层下大脑

结构

,事实证明,其中一些

对视力也至关重要,

所以呃我一直对此

感到非常惊讶。

我们认为是我们的

思考器官的大脑

实际上确实致力于这个看似

简单的任务

,即只是看到或感知

世界当然另一方面

,当我们思考

思考时,当我们谈论思考时,

我们也是如此。 通常以高度视觉化的方式

这样做 我们谈论不透明或

朦胧

或可能晶莹剔透的想法 我们谈论

可能具有洞察力

或启发性的理论 结束,我们谈论

那些非常聪明的人,

或者如果我们想侮辱他们,我们

可能会说他们有点昏暗

,所以也许你可以在这里看到我的意思

,你可以看到

我要去哪里所以 所以视觉

是我们日常生活中

完全理所当然的东西

,我们使用诸如“眼见为实

”之

类的短语,也许就像经典电影中的新角色一样,

我们偶尔会意识到的矩阵

变得意识到

小裂缝和

结构上的裂缝和

我们视觉现实的某种连贯性,所以如果我们看

这张来自矩阵的静止图像,

我们可以看到 Neo

uh 举着四根手指,我

希望他举着的一切对你来说都是显而易见

的 在那里向上四根手指,

如果我们移到这张幻灯片,我再次问你

我在这里再次向你展示多少根手指

,答案似乎完全

显而易见,

显然有三个,但如果我们

看一个更完整的图片,

呃,我们 可以看到什么但实际上

也许答案可能是

两个很好你可能会说

很好这只是一个视觉技巧让我们

看一些更

简单

的东西让我们直截了当如果我

问你这些线条是否

完美这些线条是直的 并且

再次平行 答案似乎很明显,

显然

它们是直的和平行的,但是

如果我们看看

这些,你认为这些是

直的和

平行的吗?

看起来笔直且平行,

它们似乎在整个地方都在挥动

但如果我们将两张幻灯片叠加在一起,

我们可以看到实际上两组

线

都是笔直且完全

平行的,

所以这是一个众所周知的错觉,

它被称为咖啡馆 墙错觉

,之所以这么叫是

因为这个人,

英国著名心理学家理查德格雷戈里

几十年前的一天在布里斯托尔吃午饭

他瞥了一眼,看到这个,呃,

这个特殊的咖啡馆墙壁实际上

注意到了它所描述的幻觉,

然后研究了它

实际上这里离家更近了一点,所以这里有一个

放大版的咖啡馆墙壁

幻觉,

呃,我 相信你可以在墨尔本

的这座建筑的一侧看到

好吧,

所以也许看到并不总是相信

,对我们如何看待事物有很大

影响的

事情之一就是我们看到它们的背景,

所以如果我们 看看

这张幻灯片上的这两个橙色圆盘

这是另一种众所周知的错觉,

称为艾宾浩斯错觉,

我们可以看到右边的

那个看起来比较

大,左边的那个看起来比较小,

但当然它们 我们将在

这些

有点小的灰色小灰色

圆盘的背景下看右边的那个,而左边的

那个是我们在这些更大的圆盘的背景下看的

,如果我们采取 我们

可以看到的

是,事实上这两个光盘

的大小完全相同,

所以事实

证明,上下文可以对我们看待事物的方式产生一些非常强大的

影响,

我们经常谈论

那些看到黑色事物的人 和白色

,我们之所以这么说,

是因为黑色和白色看起来确实

如此截然相反,你

不可能争辩说

你知道黑色是白色或

白色是黑色,

但让我们看看这里的

插图,它是

由一位名叫爱德华·哈德尔森的视觉科学家开发的非常聪明的演示,

所以我们在这里看到的是一个黑白棋

盘格,如果我问你

这两个正方形中哪个更暗,

正方形

a 或正方形 b,那么哪个看起来更暗,

我希望你 我会

同意 a 显然看起来有点暗,

但是如果我们

更仔细地看这个插图,我们会看到

我们正在看

b 在

这个

大的绿色圆柱所投射的阴影中 呃,

所以让我们通过去掉包括阴影在内的其余图形的视觉上下文来看看 a 是否真的比 b 更暗,

所以让我们把

左边的左边放在右边

的两个正方形周围的一些上下文中

,我们 去掉最后一点

,我们可以看到实际上 a 和 b

的亮度完全相同,

所以我们实际上可以争辩说黑色是

白色,白色是黑色

,它不在,这不是一个完全

愚蠢的事情,

呃说和 只是为了告诉你

这里没有真正的扑克牌

继续看 a 和 b 让我们

把上下文放回去,当我们添加

阴影和图形的其余部分时,

你可以看到当我们添加上下文时

添加 再次回到上下文,

a 看起来与 b 完全不同,好吧,

所以我们可以从这个 uh 中看到,

上下文可以

对我们从视觉上看待事物的方式产生非常强大的影响,

那么我们如何看待思想和信仰的世界呢?

还有p

在那个更抽象的

认知领域产生了巨大的影响,

我将通过谈论

这两位心理学家进行的一项非常著名的研究来说明这一点

几年后这项研究是在这项研究发表给

呃赢得诺贝尔奖多年之后,所以早在

1970 年代初期,

卡尼曼和特沃斯基就梦想着这个

场景

,正如你所看到

的,这与我们的情况非常相关

今天发现自己

,他们要求研究

参与者做的

是想象

他们在新西兰的这个国家

正在为一种

特别

致命的病毒的爆发做准备,人们被告知

如果不采取紧急措施,

我们将 失去 600 条

生命,然后参与者被

要求做出这个

选择,他们被要求选择

我们应该采用

方案 a 还是方案 b,所以 亲,因此该

计划可能与

替代疫苗有关,也

可能与锁定

协议有关,等等呃一些

减少病毒影响的程序,

如果我们执行

200 人的计划,人们被告知的情况很好 将会得救,

而如果我们选择方案 b,则

有三分之一的概率

会拯救 600 人,而三分之二的

概率

不会拯救任何人,所以当

面临这个选择时,绝大多数

72% 的人选择了方案 a,

他们 更喜欢拯救

这 200 条生命的确定性,但是

kahneman 和 tversky 也做的

是招募第二组

参与者

,第二组第二组被

给予这个

选择,他们被告知如果我们采用

计划,

400 人将失去生命

而如果我们采用方案 b,当面临这些选择时,我们有

三分之一的概率没有人

会死

,还有三分之二的概率有 600

人会

在面临这些选择

时死亡 ge 多数事实上,

略多于多数的

人选择了程序 b,但如果你仔细观察

这些

替代方案,甚至不那么

仔细地观察,你会发现事实上

信息完全相同,

失去 600 条生命中的 400 400 条与挽救生命

完全相同 600 条生命中的 200 条和

pro

程序 b 中的信息实际上也是相同的,

所以我们可以从这个 uh 中

看到,如果我们在不同的上下文中在不同的框架中呈现 uh 基本

相同的短语

相同的信息,

我们可以产生根本性的变化

在人们的偏好

和判断中,所以如果我们

提出是的,我们

可以通过改变我们是否

拯救生命或失去生命的方式呈现信息来产生这些非常强烈的变化,

所以我们会在这里

举行一场选举 几周后,我知道 11

月初美国将举行另一场选举

,当然,如果这些选举中的任何候选人

政党 效果赢得了中或

70% 的多数,他们会非常

高兴和欣喜若狂

,所以我们可以从中

看到,这些背景效应可以

产生非常强大和非常戏剧性的

效果

即使您在判断

看似简单直接的

事物(例如某事物有多亮、某事物有多大、

无论我们是否有一条直线)

以及

当我们对哪些事物做出这些复杂

的高级判断时,也可能对感知至关重要

面对全球大流行

,我们应该做些什么所以我们可以从中学到什么我们

可以从中得出什么结论

此时您可能会有点担心

并想好

我们能否真正相信自己的

看法和判断

是 我们对世界的看法真的

受制于

我们碰巧发现自己在

你身上的特定和特定环境的摆布,可能会担心并感觉像 wb

yates 你'

因为你认为你可以清楚地看到的东西

以及你的

信念和判断的基础也许突然

之间不再那么

坚定,也许它们

突然变得更加脆弱

并且可能容易发生

很好地瓦解我认为这将是

一个非常负面的结论,但我和

我认为有一种更

积极的方式来思考

环境如何影响

我们的看法和判断,

我想 说明

通过暂时离开

心理学并进入

物理学领域

,当然我们问题的第二部分

实际上是关于

世界本质的,当然

关于世界本质的问题

确实是 真的是在

物理学的领域

而不是心理学的领域所以

嗯回到 2016 年,这个人在这里

carla rivelli

写了一本引人入胜的书,叫做现实

不是我 看来,

拉维利在本书中提出的观点之一是,我们可以

从多个角度看待现实,

因此我们有我们日常的视角

,我们有一个固体物体的世界,

物体在那里看起来是固体的,它们

存在于特定

地点的特定位置 在空间

和特定时间点的位置,但

拉维利指出,

当我们开始考虑

几乎无法想象的

微小空间尺度和基本粒子与每个粒子相互作用的短暂时间尺度

上发生的事情时,这种情况就完全崩溃了

当然,

当我们

考虑到恒星和整个星系可能形成的几乎难以想象的

大时间尺度时,我们对时间和空间的看法也完全不同,

所以我们可以

从多个角度看待现实,

所以我认为

想结束的信息是,

当我们处理一个非常大的问题时,

比如我们是否看到了这个世界

如果我们真的想获得一个更

完整的

呃更好和更完整的答案,我们几乎不可能从一个单一的角度来获得完整的答案

需要

走出我们自己的

特定背景筒仓,

而那个筒仓

可能是你自己的学术或

科学学科

,当然

我们现在生活

在非常困难和充满挑战的环境中,并且

可能非常分裂

因此,也许还有一个更

广泛的课程

要在这里学习,以尝试

从不同的角度思考问题

,当然,不同的观点

可能是

与政治光谱相反的人的观点

到您所在的位置,或者

可能是来自不同

社会或文化

群体的人,心理学家将此称为

透视

,将看到某事物的能力

视为 它可能会从不同的

角度来看

,最近透视神经科学

已经

取得了一些非常有趣的进步,通过

观察

当你想象

从不同的角度看一个视觉对象时变得活跃的大脑区域,或者如果你

想象一下

从你目前持有的信念中采用不同的信念

,有趣的

是,当

你做这两件事时,当你

想象一个不同的视觉观点

并且你想象一个不同的认知

观点时,一些非常常见的领域会被点亮,

所以这就是观点 考虑到它,它

在儿童时期很早就出现

了,大约在

两岁到五岁

之间

一些对

我们非常有益和有帮助的东西,可以

让我们在

成年人以及当

我们 孩子们

,那就是,非常感谢你的

聆听